JAMA 2002 Bhutta 728 37
JAMA 2002 Bhutta 728 37
JAMA 2002 Bhutta 728 37
T
HE INFANT MORTALITY RATE IN authors.
the United States has de- Study Selection We included case-control studies reporting cognitive and/or be-
creased from more than 12 per havioral data of children who were born preterm and who were evaluated after their
1000 live births in 1980 to ap- fifth birthday if the attrition rate was less than 30%. From the 227 reviewed studies,
proximately 7 per 1000 live births in cognitive data from 15 studies and behavioral data from 16 studies were selected.
1998. This reduction in mortality has oc- Data Extraction Data on population demographics, study characteristics, and cog-
curred during a period when an increas- nitive and behavioral outcomes were extracted from each study, entered in a custom-
ing percentage of children have been ized database, and reviewed twice to minimize error. Differences between the mean
born preterm (⬍37 weeks) with low cognitive scores of cases and controls were pooled. Homogeneity across studies was
birth weights (LBWs) (⬍2500 g) or very formally tested using a general variance-based method and graphically using Gal-
LBWs (⬍1500 g).1 This decrease can be braith plots. Linear meta-analysis regression models were fitted to explore the impact
attributed to improvements in postna- of birth weight and gestational age on cognitive outcomes. Study-specific relative risks
(RRs) were calculated for the incidence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
tal care provided in the delivery rooms and pooled. Quality assessment of the studies was performed based on a 10-point
and neonatal intensive care units. scale. Publication bias was examined using Begg modified funnel plots and formally
This decrease in mortality is paral- tested using the Egger weighted-linear regression method.
leled by an increasing recognition of
Data Synthesis Among 1556 cases and 1720 controls, controls had significantly
neurodevelopmental disabilities in higher cognitive scores compared with children who were born preterm (weighted mean
these children at school age. A large difference, 10.9; 95% confidence interval [CI], 9.2-12.5). The mean cognitive scores
number of children who were born with of preterm-born cases and term-born controls were directly proportional to their birth
a LBW or preterm have adverse out- weight (R2 =0.51; P⬍.001) and gestational age (R2 =0.49; P⬍.001). Age at evaluation
comes such as cerebral palsy, hydro- had no significant correlation with mean difference in cognitive scores (R2 =0.12; P=.20).
cephalus, blindness, deafness, or sei- Preterm-born children showed increases in externalizing and internalizing behaviors
zures.2-4 Multiple observational studies in 81% of studies and had more than twice the RR for developing ADHD (pooled RR,
of children who were born preterm have 2.64; 95% CI, 1.85-3.78). No differences were noted in cognition and behaviors based
on the quality of the study.
followed up cohorts from birth to
school age (ⱖ5 years) and have re- Conclusions Children who were born preterm are at risk for reduced cognitive test
ported on their cognitive and behav- scores and their immaturity at birth is directly proportional to the mean cognitive scores
at school age. Preterm-born children also show an increased incidence of ADHD and
ioral outcomes. Even in children with-
other behaviors.
out obvious neurological deficits, subtle
JAMA. 2002;288:728-737 www.jama.com
abnormalities occur, which include
Author Affiliations are listed at the end of this MBBS, DPhil, Arkansas Children’s Hospital, 800
lower cognitive test scores and in- article. Marshall St, S-431, Little Rock, AR 72202 (e-mail:
creased behavioral problems.5,6 How- Corresponding Author and Reprints: K. J. S. Anand, anandsunny@exchange.uams.edu).
728 JAMA, August 14, 2002—Vol 288, No. 6 (Reprinted) ©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
ever, some studies have found no dif- to English-language publications, and it erature.8,9,12-14 The data extracted from
ferences between preterm-born cases was supplemented by a manual search these studies were entered in an open-
and term-born controls.7 of the reference lists of all primary articles ended fashion and coding of the vari-
The magnitude of the effect of pre- and review articles. No attempt was made ables at the time of data entry was mini-
term birth on cognitive and behav- to contact the authors of any of these mized. The data from each study were
ioral outcomes at school age is un- studies. We also performed a manual reviewed twice to minimize the chances
known. The extreme variability in the search of files maintained by 2 coau- of data-entry errors. A wide array of be-
published data results from the rela- thors (P.H.C. and K.J.S.A.). haviors were assessed and various be-
tively small numbers of subjects in each We defined a priori criteria for the in- havioral methods were used in the se-
study and marked variation in the meth- clusion of studies in this meta-analysis, lected studies. For the purposes of this
ods used for their evaluation. Several selecting only those studies that in- meta-analysis, we classified the behav-
of these studies have been criticized due cluded an evaluation of concurrent con- ior of subjects into externalizing behav-
to problems with study design, non- trols. Studies were included if they (1) iors (eg, hyperactivity, delinquency) or
representative study samples, inad- had a case-control design, (2) reported internalizing behaviors (eg, anxiety, de-
equate demographic data, high attri- cognitive data, behavioral data or both, pression, phobias). Data from studies
tion rates, poor selection of control (3) performed evaluations after the fifth that used standard diagnostic criteria for
groups, the systematic exclusion of sub- birthday of the enrolled subjects, (4) had attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
groups of patients, and other con- an attrition rate (loss to follow-up) of less der (ADHD), either the Diagnostic and
cerns.8 This has led to difficulties in es- than 30%, and (5) were published in Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
timating the true effect of preterm birth 1980 or later. Studies were excluded if Third Edition (DSM-III), Diagnostic and
on cognition and behavior problems. they did not meet all of these inclusion Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Re-
We performed a meta-analysis to ar- criteria. Studies that primarily exam- vised Third Edition (DSM-III-R), or Di-
rive at a better estimate of the effect of ined LBW children were excluded be- agnostic and Statistical Manual of Men-
preterm birth on cognitive and behav- cause of the possibility that small-for- tal Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV),
ioral outcomes in school-aged chil- gestational-age term infants may be were extracted for further analysis.
dren. The 2 available meta-analyses on included in these cohorts.
this patient population were pub- If multiple studies were published Statistical Methods
lished more than 10 years ago and in- from the same cohort of subjects at dif- This meta-analysis was performed us-
vestigated neurodevelopmental out- ferent ages, only the last published re- ing STATA statistical software (Version
comes in preschool children.9,10 We port was included (unless the cogni- 7, STATA Corp, College Station, Tex).
report the first meta-analysis on the cog- tive and behavioral data were published For each study, the nonstandardized dif-
nitive and behavioral outcomes of separately, in which case both reports ference between the mean cognitive test
school-aged children who were born were included). Studies were also ex- scores of cases (preterm-born children)
preterm by combining the results from cluded if they reported outcomes on a and controls (term-born children) was
case-control studies published be- defined subgroup of the population (eg, weighted by the inverse of the variance
tween 1980 and November 2001. only cases with intraventricular hem- for this difference. These weighted mean
orrhage) or if the same cognitive test differences (WMDs) were pooled across
METHODS was not used for all subjects. From the studies to compute an overall mean cog-
Selection of Studies 227 studies retrieved and reviewed, only nitive difference between cases and con-
The guidelines published by Stroup et al11 15 studies with cognitive data and 16 trols. Cognitive scores from all studies
for the meta-analysis of observational studies with behavioral data met these were obtained from comparable tests of
studies were followed in the design, per- selection criteria. cognition (normative data from all re-
formance, and reporting of this meta- ported tests had a mean [SD] of 100 [15].
analysis. A MEDLINE search included Data Extraction Therefore, the nonstandardized WMD
the period from 1980 to November 2001 Data were entered into a customized was chosen as the cognitive outcome
and used the subject headings infant- database created for this meta-analysis, measure for combining studies in this
premature; or infant, low birth weight; and with data extracted on the study de- meta-analysis.
cognition; developmental disabilities; child sign, attrition rate, demographic vari- We used random-effects and fixed-
development; personality development; ables, geographic location, socioeco- effects least-square regression models
child development disorders; human devel- nomic status, and detailed information for combining the results in this meta-
opment; behavior; child behavior disor- on the cognitive and behavioral evalu- analysis. The fixed-effects and random-
ders; attention deficit; and disruptive behav- ations performed. Explanatory vari- effects models produced virtually iden-
ior disorders. These search terms were ables were chosen based on their sig- tical results; therefore, only the results
combined with the “explode” feature nificant association with cognitive and obtained from the random-effects mod-
when applicable, the search was limited behavioral outcomes in the published lit- els are presented.
©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, August 14, 2002—Vol 288, No. 6 729
©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, August 14, 2002—Vol 288, No. 6 731
beled high quality vs low quality (2.5 We limited our search to English- term but were small for their gesta-
[95% CI, 1.4-4.3] vs 2.8 [95% CI, 1.7- language literature because of practical tional age and were not identified in the
4.4]; P=.86). The tests using methods difficulties in abstracting data from ar- description of their cohorts.
by Begg (P=.55) and Egger et al (P=.49) ticles published in other languages. Gre- Data on birth weight and gesta-
showed no significant publication bias. goire et al51 showed that in only 1 of 36 tional age were not always reported as
However, the small number of studies consecutive meta-analyses of random- mean (SD) or range (Table 1). For these
(n=6) makes these tests unreliable. ized clinical trials, the inclusion of non– control groups, we assumed a mean
English-language articles produced re- term gestation of 40 weeks56 and a mean
COMMENT sults different from the published meta- birth weight of 3200 g (50th percen-
Our meta-analysis shows that preterm analysis (with a change in pooled odds tile for age). Cases with severe neuro-
birth is associated with lower cogni- ratio from 0.70 to 0.67). Such compari- logical and cognitive disability were ex-
tive scores and increased risks for ADHD sons have not been published for meta- cluded in all but 3 of the studies
and other behaviors at school age com- analyses of observational studies simi- included in the meta-analysis, al-
pared with term-born controls. Lower lar to ours. Despite the exclusion of non– though the exact definition of severe
cognitive scores for the cases were noted English-language articles, our meta- disability varied between these stud-
in all the studies selected for this meta- analysis contains data from many ies. The studies that included cases with
analysis, and a WMD of greater than 10 countries and no significant differences severe neurological and cognitive dis-
between the cognitive scores of cases occurred in the cognitive outcomes of ability had the highest WMD between
and controls is likely to have signifi- children born preterm in the United the cases and controls (Figure 1).27,32,35
cant educational and social conse- States vs non-US cohorts. For the sake For the purposes of our analysis, we
quences.44 Lower birth weight and ges- of completeness, a repeat MEDLINE assumed that the cognitive scores from
tational age were significantly correlated search found only 4 non–English- the various tests were comparable be-
with decreases in cognitive test scores, language articles (with online abstracts cause of similar normative data for all
highlighting the developmental vul- in English) that were designed as case- the cognitive tests used (mean [SD], 100
nerability of the immature brain. Super- control studies with cognitive and/or be- [15]). Similarly, we assumed that the
imposed on this vulnerability are the havioral data for preterm or term-born different standardized assessments used
factors associated with severity of ill- school-aged children.52-55 Because of the for the diagnosis of ADHD had com-
ness in preterm neonates, their physi- limited information contained in each ab- parable sensitivity and specificity (DSM-
ological instability and exposure to early stract, we cannot say whether these stud- III, DSM-III-R, and DSM-IV). These as-
adverse experiences, which may have ies fulfilled all the inclusion and exclu- sumptions overlook the subtle
a persistent impact on brain develop- sion criteria for this meta-analysis. All 4 differences between the cognitive and
ment leading to these cognitive and of these studies reached conclusions that behavioral tests used, as well as the vari-
behavioral outcomes. are consistent with the results of our ability in administering these tests.
The results of this meta-analysis must meta-analysis. Based on these consider- The stringent application of our se-
be viewed in the light of its limita- ations, we propose that including the re- lection criteria resulted in the exclu-
tions. Multiple studies conducted over sults from non–English-language stud- sion of studies with poor methodo-
the decades have also demonstrated the ies would not have altered our results or logic quality (eg, attrition rate ⬎30%)
significant impact of demographic and conclusions. and poor generalizability (eg, reports of
environmental factors (such as age, sex, Furthermore, the included studies subgroup analyses57). Assessment of
race, and socioeconomic status) on the were published in an era when rapid ad- study quality for meta-analyses of ran-
trajectory of cognitive and behavioral vances were occurring in the field of fe- domized clinical trials has been ques-
development in both preterm39,45-48 and tal medicine and perinatology. There- tioned and may give misleading results
full-term12,49 infants. In a recent study fore, the care provided to these infants in meta-analyses.58 Specific criteria are
of 118 children at age 10 years who was not uniform and must have evolved widely accepted for assessing the qual-
were born preterm, family factors were over the period studied. The selected ity of randomized controlled trials (eg,
stronger predictors of school perfor- studies differed from one another in their randomization, double-blinding, drop-
mance than were perinatal complica- baseline characteristics, such as mean outs, or allocation concealment); how-
tions.50 All the studies included in our birth weight and gestational age. All neo- ever, similar criteria have not been de-
meta-analysis featured cases and con- nates with a gestational age of less than veloped for observational studies. We
trols matched for 1 or more demo- 37 weeks were defined as cases, and the devised a quality assessment tool spe-
graphic variables. Thus, we were un- data obtained from those who were born cifically for this meta-analysis, using the
able to determine the specific impact of full-term but were small for their ges- objective criteria listed in Table 1. The
demographic variables on the mea- tational age were excluded. It is pos- quality of the studies included in this
sured cognitive and behavioral out- sible, however, that some studies may meta-analysis was assessed indepen-
comes. have included those who were born full- dently by 2 of the authors, but showed
©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, August 14, 2002—Vol 288, No. 6 733
734 JAMA, August 14, 2002—Vol 288, No. 6 (Reprinted) ©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
736 JAMA, August 14, 2002—Vol 288, No. 6 (Reprinted) ©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
11. Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, et al, for the low-up of children born before 29 gestational weeks: 52. Evers-Embden B, Scholte EM. Leerprestaties en
Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemi- health, cognitive development, behaviour and school neurologische status praesens van prematuren, pre-
ology (MOOSE) group. Meta-analysis of observa- achievement. Acta Paediatr. 1999;88:557-562. matue-dysmaturen en dysmaturen op 8-jarige leeftijd.
tional studies in epidemiology: a proposal for report- 33. Wolke D, Meyer R. Cognitive status, language at- Tijdschr Kindergeneeskunde. 1983;51:85-94.
ing. JAMA. 2000;283:2008-2012. tainment, and prereading skills of 6-year-old very pre- 53. Lubetzky O, Weitzman A, Gilat I, Tyano S. Pre-
12. Aylward GP. The relationship between environ- term children and their peers: the Bavarian Longitu- mature birth and cognitive functioning in adoles-
mental risk and developmental outcome. J Dev Be- dinal Study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1999;41:94- cence [in Hebrew]. Harefuah. 1999;137:380-383.
hav Pediatr. 1992;13:222-229. 109. 54. Finnstrom O, Leijon I, Samuelsson S, et al. Skols-
13. Ornstein M, Ohlsson A, Edmonds J, Asztalos E. 34. Saigal S, Hoult LA, Streiner DL, Stoskopf BL, Rosen- varigheter vanliga hos barn med mycket lag fodelse-
Neonatal follow-up of very low birthweight/ baum PL. School difficulties at adolescence in a re- vikt: extra uppmarksamhet och stod behovs vid skol-
extremely low birthweight infants to school age: a criti- gional cohort of children who were extremely low birth start. Lakartidningen. 2000;97:3492-3495.
cal overview. Acta Paediatr. 1991;80:741-748. weight. Pediatrics. 2000;105:325-331. 55. Burguet A, Monnet E, Roth P, et al. Neurodevel-
14. Verloove-Vanhorick SP, Veen S, Ens-Dokkum MH, 35. Taylor HG, Klein N, Minich NM, Hack M. Middle- opmental outcome of premature infants born at less
Schreuder AM, Brand R, Ruys JH. Sex difference in dis- school-age outcomes in children with very low birth- than 33 weeks of gestational age and not cerebral palsy
ability and handicap at five years of age in children born weight. Child Dev. 2000;71:1495-1511. at the age of 5 years [in French]. Arch Pediatr. 2000;
at very short gestation. Pediatrics. 1994;93:576-579. 36. Rickards AL, Kelly EA, Doyle LW, Callanan C. Cog- 7:357-368.
15. Greenland S. Meta-analysis. In: Rothman K, Green- nition, academic progress, behavior and self-concept 56. Avery M, Richardson D. History and epidemiol-
land S, eds. Modern Epidemiology. 2nd ed. Philadel- at 14 years of very low birth weight children. J Dev ogy. In: Taeusch H, Ballard R, eds. Avery’s Diseases
phia, Pa: Lippincott-Raven; 1998:643-673. Behav Pediatr. 2001;22:11-18. of the Newborn. 7th ed. Philadelphia, Pa: WB Saun-
16. Laird NM, Mosteller F. Some statistical methods 37. Szatmari P, Saigal S, Rosenbaum P, Campbell D, ders; 1998:1-12.
for combining experimental results. Int J Technol As- King S. Psychiatric disorders at five years among chil- 57. Peterson BS, Vohr B, Staib LH, et al. Regional brain
sess Health Care. 1990;6:5-30. dren with birthweights less than 1000 g: a regional per- volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive out-
17. Galbraith RF. A note on graphical presentation of spective. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1990;32:954-962. come in preterm infants. JAMA. 2000;284:1939-
estimated odds ratios from several clinical trials. Stat 38. Ross G, Lipper EG, Auld PA. Educational status 1947.
Med. 1988;7:889-894. and school-related abilities of very low birth weight 58. Juni P. The hazards of scoring the quality of clini-
18. Thompson SG, Sharp SJ. Explaining heterogene- premature children. Pediatrics. 1991;88:1125-1134. cal trials for meta-analysis. JAMA. 1999;282:1054-
ity in meta-analysis: a comparison of methods. Stat 39. Hack M, Breslau N, Aram D, Weissman B, Klein 1060.
Med. 1999;18:2693-2708. N, Borawski-Clark E. The effect of very low birth weight 59. Chaikind S, Corman H. The impact of low birth-
19. Begg CB, Mazumdar M. Operating characteris- and social risk on neurocognitive abilities at school age. weight on special education costs. J Health Econ. 1991;
tics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Bio- J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1992;13:412-420. 10:291-311.
metrics. 1994;50:1088-1101. 40. Botting N, Powls A, Cooke RW, Marlow N. Atten- 60. Bhutta AT, Anand KJS. Abnormal cognition and
20. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder tion deficit hyperactivity disorders and other psychiat- behavior in preterm neonates linked to smaller brain
C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphi- ric outcomes in very low birthweight children at 12 years. volumes. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:129-132.
cal test. BMJ. 1997;315:629-634. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:931-941. 61. Stewart AL, Rifkin L, Amess PN, et al. Brain struc-
21. Lin LI. A concordance correlation coefficient to 41. Whitfield MF, Grunau RV, Holsti L. Extremely pre- ture and neurocognitive and behavioural function in
evaluate reproducibility. Biometrics. 1989;45:255- mature (⬍ or=800 g) schoolchildren: multiple areas adolescents who were born very preterm. Lancet. 1999;
268. of hidden disability. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 353:1653-1657.
22. Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistical methods for as- 1997;77:F85-F90. 62. Rabinowicz T, de Courten-Myers GM, Petetot JM,
sessing agreement between two methods of clinical 42. Horwood LJ, Mogridge N, Darlow BA. Cogni- Xi G, de los Reyes E. Human cortex development: es-
measurement. Lancet. 1986;1:307-310. tive, educational, and behavioural outcomes at 7 to timates of neuronal numbers indicate major loss late
23. Lloyd BW, Wheldall K, Perks D. Controlled study 8 years in a national very low birthweight cohort. Arch during gestation. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1996;
of intelligence and school performance of very low- Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1998;79:F12-F20. 55:320-328.
birthweight children from a defined geographical area. 43. Stevenson CJ, Blackburn P, Pharoah PO. Longi- 63. Mitani A, Watanabe M, Kataoka K. Functional
Dev Med Child Neurol. 1988;30:36-42. tudinal study of behaviour disorders in low birth- change of NMDA receptors related to enhancement
24. Portnoy S, Callias M, Wolke D, Gamsu H. Five- weight infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1999; of susceptibility to neurotoxicity in the developing pon-
year follow-up study of extremely low-birthweight in- 81:F5-F9. tine nucleus. J Neurosci. 1998;18:7941-7952.
fants. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1988;30:590-598. 44. McCarton CM, Brooks-Gunn J, Wallace IF, et al. 64. Breslau N, Chilcoat HD, Johnson EO, Andreski P,
25. McDonald MA, Sigman M, Ungerer JA. Intelli- Results at age 8 years of early intervention for low- Lucia VC. Neurologic soft signs and low birthweight:
gence and behavior problems in 5-year-olds in rela- birth-weight premature infants: the Infant Health and their association and neuropsychiatric implications. Biol
tion to representational abilities in the second year of Development Program. JAMA. 1997;277:126-132. Psychiatry. 2000;47:71-79.
life. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1989;10:86-91. 45. Escalona SK. Babies at double hazard: early de- 65. Singer LT, Salvator A, Guo S, Collin M, Lilien L,
26. Smith AE, Knight-Jones EB. The abilities of very velopment of infants at biologic and social risk. Pedi- Baley J. Maternal psychological distress and parent-
low-birthweight children and their classroom con- atrics. 1982;70:670-676. ing stress after the birth of a very low-birth-weight
trols. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1990;32:590-601. 46. Klebanov PK, Brooks-Gunn J, McCormick MC. infant. JAMA. 1999;281:799-805.
27. Teplin SW, Burchinal M, Johnson-Martin N, Classroom behavior of very low birth weight elemen- 66. Robson AL. Low birth weight and parenting stress
Humphry RA, Kraybill EN. Neurodevelopmental, health, tary school children. Pediatrics. 1994;94:700-708. during early childhood. J Pediatr Psychol. 1997;22:
and growth status at age 6 years of children with birth 47. Grantham-McGregor SM, Lira PI, Ashworth A, 297-311.
weights less than 1001 grams. Pediatrics. 1991;118: Morris SS, Assuncao AM. The development of low birth 67. Rasco L, Skinner RD, Garcia-Rill E. Effect of age
768-777. weight term infants and the effects of the environ- on sensory gating of the sleep state-dependent P1/
28. Levy-Shiff R, Einat G, Mogilner MB, Lerman M, ment in northeast Brazil. Pediatrics. 1998;132:661- P50 midlatency auditory evoked potential. Sleep Res
Krikler R. Biological and environmental correlates of de- 666. Online [serial online]. 2000;3:97-105.
velopmental outcome of prematurely born infants in 48. Resnick MB, Gueorguieva RV, Carter RL, et al. The 68. Gardner JM, Karmel BZ. Attention and arousal in
early adolescence. J Pediatr Psychol. 1994;19:63-78. impact of low birth weight, perinatal conditions, and so- preterm and full-term neonates. In: Field T, Sostek A,
29. Hall A, McLeod A, Counsell C, Thomson L, Mutch ciodemographic factors on educational outcome in kin- eds. Infants Born at Risk: Physiological, Perceptual and
L. School attainment, cognitive ability and motor func- dergarten [serial online]. Pediatrics. 1999;104:e74. Cognitive Processes. New York, NY: Grune & Strat-
tion in a total Scottish very-low-birthweight popula- 49. Sameroff AJ, Seifer R, Barocas R, Zax M, Greenspan ton; 1983:69-98.
tion at eight years: a controlled study. Dev Med Child S. Intelligence quotient scores of 4-year-old children: 69. Gladman G, Chiswick ML. Skin conductance and
Neurol. 1995;37:1037-1050. social-environmental risk factors. Pediatrics. 1987;79: arousal in the newborn. AJDC. 1990;65:1063-1066.
30. Sommerfelt K, Ellertsen B, Markestad T. Parental 343-350. 70. Langkamp DL, Brazy JE. Risk for later school prob-
factors in cognitive outcome of non-handicapped low 50. Gross SJ, Mettelman BB, Dye TD, Slagle TA. Im- lems in preterm children who do not cooperate for pre-
birthweight infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. pact of family structure and stability on academic out- school developmental testing. Pediatrics. 1999;135:
1995;73:F135-F142. come in preterm children at 10 years of age. Pediat- 756-760.
31. Botting N, Powls A, Cooke RW, Marlow N. Cog- rics. 2001;138:169-175. 71. Brooks-Gunn J, McCarton CM, Casey PH, et al.
nitive and educational outcome of very-low- 51. Gregoire G, Derderian F, Le Lorier J. Selecting the Early intervention in low-birth-weight premature in-
birthweight children in early adolescence. Dev Med language of the publications included in a meta- fants: results through age 5 years from the Infant Health
Child Neurol. 1998;40:652-660. analysis: is there a Tower of Babel bias? J Clin Epide- and Development Program. JAMA. 1994;272:1257-
32. Stjernqvist K, Svenningsen NW. Ten-year fol- miol. 1995;48:159-163. 1262.
©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. (Reprinted) JAMA, August 14, 2002—Vol 288, No. 6 737