Jsep Article p311
Jsep Article p311
Jsep Article p311
The purpose of this study was to quantitatively review the body of research
on exercise and global self-esteem (GSE). This review focuses specifically
on studies using adults and also incorporates both published and unpublished
works. Computer and manual searches identified 113 studies matching the
selection criteria. Each study was coded according to 20 study features. A total
of 128 effect sizes (d) were derived. As indicated by effect-size magnitude,
participation in exercise brought about a small change in GSE (dd = +0.23).
Change in physical fitness and type of program were significant moderators of
the effect of exercise on GSE. Larger effect sizes were observed for those who
experienced significant changes in physical fitness and those participating in
exercise or lifestyle programs as opposed to skills training.
The self is one of the most widely utilized psychological constructs in con-
temporary society (Harter, 1999; Hattie, 1992). In Western culture, the academic
literature and popular press presuppose that everyone not only has a sense of self
but that the self we have is a social phenomenon (Cooley, 1902; James, 1890/1950;
Mead, 1934). Stated differently, who we are is thought to be reciprocally influenced
by our relationships with others, providing the key to understanding how and why
we think, feel, and behave as we do, as well as the impetus for enriching our lives
and relationships.
Researchers interested in exercise and physical activity have also recognized
the importance of self-related phenomena. This is because exercise participation is
linked to mental and psychological benefits (Gauvin, Spence, & Anderson, 1999;
Landers & Arent, 2001). Involvement in regular exercise and physical activity
programs that improve skill, knowledge, fitness, and health are linked to enhanced
self-perceptions (Fox, 1997). These changes may then generalize to more favor-
able views about the self, leading to an improved sense of well-being (Berger &
McInman, 1993; Morgan, 1985).
1Facultyof Physical Education and Recreation, & Alberta Centre for Active Living,
E-424 Van Vliet, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, T6G 2H9 Canada; 2Dept. of
Health and Sport Studies, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242.
311
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
312 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 313
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
314 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
Third, similar to the case for initial physical fitness level, initial self-esteem
level should also serve as a moderator of exercise on GSE. Thus, participants with
initially lower GSE scores should experience the most change in self-esteem as a
result of exercise and changes in fitness.
Finally, since training volume and dose are related to changes in physical
fitness (McArdle, Katch, & Katch, 1986), aspects of the exercise program such as
frequency, duration, intensity, and length of the program should be related to changes
in GSE. Specifically, programs that take place over a longer period, with exercise
bouts of higher frequency, intensity, and duration, should lead to larger changes
in GSE vs. programs of shorter length with lower doses of exercise.
Despite the vast number of studies exploring the GSE and exercise relation-
ship, it should be clear that while the potential benefit of physical activity on GSE
has long been espoused in the sport and exercise literature (Folkins & Sime, 1981;
Hughes, 1984), the foregoing studies and their limitations have put into question
the exact magnitude of change in GSE that results from participation in physical
activity. Therefore, the primary purpose of this paper was to take the limitations of
previous research into consideration and present the findings of a more comprehen-
sive meta-analysis in order to explore the effect of exercise on GSE. To accomplish
this purpose, the current meta-analysis included published and unpublished studies
(e.g., dissertations, theses) and also explored the effects of other forms of exercise
(e.g., weight training) in addition to aerobic exercise on GSE.
Method
Literature Search and Study Selection
Search Strategy. We conducted computer-based information searches in
Current Contents (July 1993–Sept. 2001), Dissertation Abstracts (1865–Dec.
2003), Medline (1966–Dec. 2003), PsycINFO (1960–Dec. 2003), and Sport Discus
(1975–Dec. 2003). Also, manual searches were done through Current Contents
(1987–June 1993), Physical Education Index (1983–1993), and Completed Research
in Health, Physical Education, and Recreation (1968–1995). Reference lists of all
major reviews and primary studies were checked for unidentified references. To find
relevant studies on exercise and GSE, the keywords used in the searches included,
but were not limited to, terms such as: exercise, physical activity, self-esteem,
self-concept, and self-perception. The search was limited by language in that only
studies published in English were included. In an attempt to obtain unpublished
material, we emailed researchers with recent publications on the topic and asked if
they would share any unpublished manuscripts or data in their possession. The final
cutoff date for articles to be included in this review was December 2003.
Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria. The criteria for including studies in the final
sample were as follows: (a) Information had to be reported in a study format, either
published or unpublished, as opposed to a case study or editorial; (b) the dependent
variable had to be a global measure of self-concept, self-esteem, or self-percep-
tion; (c) the independent variable had to include exercise/physical activity as an
intervention; (d) because of the large number of studies found in our initial search
on the topic, we decided to limit our focus to adults and thus required that study
participants be at least 18 years of age or that the mean age of the sample likewise
be 18 years or older; (e) studies had to include a nonexercising control group or a
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 315
control group that was maintaining its usual level of physical activity; (f) the train-
ing program had to last more than 1 week; and (g) outcomes of the intervention
had to be quantified and sample size be provided.
Included Studies. The original search resulted in 426 potential studies. Based
on our inclusion criteria, 113 studies (42 published, 71 unpublished) revealing 128
effect sizes were included in the analysis.3 In total, 7724 participants were included
in these studies.
Coding of Studies
Once studies were identified for inclusion, they were collected, coded for
moderator effects, and an estimate of treatment effect was calculated.
Hypothesized Moderator Variables. Based on the original hypotheses
proposed for the EXSEM (Sonstroem & Morgan, 1989) and other writings of
Sonstroem (1984, 1997a, 1998), we hypothesized that changes in physical fitness,
initial self-esteem level, initial physical fitness level, and dose of exercise (fre-
quency, duration, intensity, and length of program) would explain any variation in
the effect of exercise on GSE.
Additional Moderator Variables. Further, for each study we recorded the
characteristics of the sample (age, sex, population, health status), methodology
(self-term, questionnaire, participant assignment, control condition), and treatment
(type of intervention, mode of exercise, type of physical fitness measure). In order
to address concerns about publication bias, we also coded the studies for publication
status, i.e., published vs. unpublished. Finally, based on a subjective assessment
of internal validity, we rated each study on a scale from 1 (poor) to 3 (good) for
methodological quality.
The factors considered for this rating were assignment of participants to condi-
tions, dropout rate, dose of exercise, and assessment of physical fitness. This rating
was applied in a hierachical fashion. First we assessed the method of assignment of
participants to conditions and the dropout rate. If participants were not randomly
assigned to conditions and/or the dropout rate was greater than 50%, the study was
rated as poor. Second, if these conditions were met and a specific dose of exercise
was required and/or the physical fitness of participants was measured before and
after the intervention, the study was rated as good. Otherwise the study was rated
as being only fair. For example, studies were rated as good if random assignment
was used to allocate participants to groups, the dropout rate was less than 50%, and
assessments of physical fitness were conducted before and after the intervention.
Publication status and study quality were included as methodology moderators.
Coding was undertaken independently by two coders. Their initial agreement rate
was 84%. Disagreements between the two coders were resolved through discussion
and further review of the disputed studies.
Effect Size Computation. Hedges and Olkin’s (1985) procedure to derive
and analyze effect sizes was used. Specifically, the effect size, g, was defined as
the difference between the means of the experimental group and the control group
divided by the pooled within-group standard deviation. The pooled standard devia-
tion is considered to be a representative indicator of within-study variance (Hedges
& Olkin, 1985). If descriptive statistics were not available, then estimates of g were
calculated from other statistics such as t, F, or p (see Dunlap, Cortina, Vaslow, &
Burke, 1996; Glass, McGaw, & Smith, 1981; Ray & Shadish, 1996).
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
316 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
Effect sizes are positive if exercisers exhibit higher levels of GSE. The effect
sizes were corrected, d, for the bias from g’s overestimate of the population effect
size for small samples (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). Statistical significance of d was
determined by the presence or absence of 0 within the 95% CI for d. The absence
of 0 from the CI indicates a significant effect size. A d of 1 indicates a change in
magnitude equivalent to 1 SD. According to Cohen (1992), effect sizes can be
categorized as small (dd = 0.2), medium (d = 0.5), or large (d = 0.8).
Unit of Analysis. The unit of analysis was the study finding. Multiple effect
sizes within outcomes were taken from studies only if different samples were used
to produce these effect sizes (e.g., males vs. females).
Missing Data. An effect size of 0 was imputed if complete descriptive (i.e.,
means, variances, and sample size) and/or inferential statistics were unavailable and
it was clear that there was no treatment effect (i.e., Meanexp – Meancont = 0). Also,
if a result was only described as not significant and n and/or direction of change
could not be determined, then 0 was substituted for the effect size (see Lou, Abrami,
Spence, et al., 1996). However, if a result was described as not significant, and n
and direction of change could be determined, then the effect size was calculated
based on the value of t for p = .05 and divided by a factor of 2 (Sedlmeier & Giger-
enzer, 1989). Overall, 0 was substituted in 6 cases (5%) and tt/2 was substituted in
4 cases (3.4%).
Combining Effect Sizes. Because a great effort was made to identify and
obtain every study conducted on the topic, a fixed-effects model (Hedges & Olkin,
1985) was used when combining effect sizes. That is, we assumed that the stud-
ies in our meta-analysis constituted the universe of studies. When calculating the
overall average effect size for each outcome, d+, we weighted each study’s effect
size by the reciprocal of its variance before averaging it with other effect sizes. This
procedure typically gives additional weight to effect sizes that come from studies
with larger sample sizes (Hedges, 1994).
Data Analysis
To detect whether the studies shared a common effect size, we tested the
homogeneity of the set of effect sizes with a homogeneity statistic, Qt, which has
an approximate chi-square distribution with k – 1 degree of freedom, where k is
the number of effect sizes (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). If the resulting chi-square was
significant, the effect sizes were determined to be heterogeneous. In such a case,
Hedges and Olkin (1985) recommend that the total homogeneity statistic be par-
titioned into a between-classes effect and a test of the homogeneity of the effect
sizes be conducted within each class. The between-classes effect was estimated by
Qb, which has an approximate chi-square distribution with p – 1 degree of freedom,
where p is the number of classes. A large value of Qb indicated that there were
significant differences among the classes of effect sizes. As suggested by Hedges
(1994), for any variable with more than two levels, statistically significant between-
classes effects were followed-up with fixed-effects contrasts (Bonferroni method).
The homogeneity of the effect sizes within each class was estimated by Qwi, which
has an approximate chi-square distribution with m – 1 degree of freedom, where m
is the number of effect sizes in the class.
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 317
Results
Based on the average weighted effect size (d+ = 0.23, SE = 0.02), adults in
the studies in this analysis experienced approximately 1/4 of a standard deviation
increase in GSE when they participated in physical activity or exercise. While
this change was deemed small, it was significantly different from zero (95% CI,
.18/.28) and the effect sizes were found to be homogeneous, Qt(127) = 144.39, p =
.14. This weighted effect size was similar in magnitude to the unweighted median
effect size (MM = 0.24) and the unweighted mean effect size (d = 0.27). Because
of concerns about the imputing of zero for those cases in which the findings were
nonsignificant, and since it was not possible to calculate an effect size, we also
calculated the average weighted effect size with those cases removed. Again, there
was very little difference between the effect size with zero removed (dd0– = 0.24, SE
= 0.02, 95% CI, .19/.28) and the average weighted effect size.
Even though the effect sizes for GSE were homogeneous, we proceeded with
an analysis of both the hypothesized and additional moderators as Rosenthal (1995)
recommends.4 Because the additional moderators were tested in an exploratory
fashion, we used a Bonferroni inequality to obtain a more conservative alpha of
.004 (i.e., .05/13 categorical tests) to determine statistical significance. Tables 1
and 2 provide the average weighted effect size for each level of each moderating
variable, along with indicators of between-group differences (Qb) and within-group
homogeneity (Qw).
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
318 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
Variable Qb k d+ SE 95% CI Qw
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 319
Variable Qb k d+ SE 95%CI Qw
Sample Variables
Age 2.01 113.63
Young 58 .24 .03 .17/.30 64.41
Middle-age 32 .30 .06 .19/.41 33.64
Older adults 18 .18 .08 .03/.33 15.58
Sex 1.00 143.31
Women 57 .21 .04 .14/.29 76.18*
Men 21 .28 .05 .17/.38 15.57
Mixed sample 49 .22 .04 .15/.30 51.56
Population 8.02 136.29
Public 48 .32 .04 .24/.41 44.00
Students 55 .19 .03 .12/.25 64.43
Patients 15 .28 .09 .10/.46 20.54
Other 9 .12 .09 –.06/.30 7.31
Health status 0.64 143.67
Healthy 97 .22 .03 .17/.27 111.13
Physically unhealthy 15 .23 .08 .06/.40 8.33
Psychologically unhealthy 14 .29 .08 .13/.46 24.21*
Methodological Variables
Self term 0.05 142.29
Self-esteem 44 .23 .04 .14/.31 49.48
Self-concept 83 .24 .04 .18/.30 92.81
Scale 3.52 140.87
R-SES 27 .18 .05 .08/.28 20.57
TSCS 67 .24 .03 .18/.31 65.97
SCS 8 .39 .10 .18/.59 13.22
Other 26 .20 .05 .10/.31 41.11*
Publication status 0.22 144.17
Published 48 .24 .04 .17/.32 40.59
Unpublished 80 .22 .03 .16/.28 103.58*
Control group 0.16 144.22
No treatment 110 .23 .03 .18/.28 121.33
Other treatment 13 .24 .08 .07.40 17.83
Some exercise 5 .18 .14 –.09/.44 5.06
Group assignment 6.60 137.82
Random 54 .26 .04 .18/.34 54.63
Matching 9 .48 .11 .26/.71 6.05
Nonequivalent 65 .20 .03 .14/.26 77.14
(continued)
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
320 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
Table 2 (Continued)
Variable Qb k d+ SE 95%CI Qw
Published Unpublished
Study quality (n = 48) (n = 80) χ2 (1)
Poor 9 46 24.89***
Fair 28 23 0.49
Good 11 11 0.00
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 321
publication status and study quality, χ2 (2, N = 128) = 18.54, p < .001, with effect
sizes from poor quality studies being more likely to remain unpublished, χ2 (1, N
= 55) = 24.89, p < .001.
Treatment Variables. Type of program, Qb (2) = 14.80, p < .001, was a
significant moderator of GSE. Those participating in exercise (dd = 0.26) or lifestyle
enrichment programs (dd = 0.36) experienced a larger increase in GSE than those
in skills training activities (d = –0.03).5 Also, the effect sizes for both exercise and
lifestyle programs were larger than zero. The other variables, exercise mode and
type of fitness change, were not significant moderators of GSE.
Discussion
The current findings demonstrate that exercise participation leads to small
yet significant increases in GSE. The effect sizes in this case were homogeneous.
Thus while there is a positive and reliable effect of exercise on self-esteem, this
relationship is small at the global level. These results are in contrast with McDonald
and Hodgdon’s (1991) meta-analysis which found that exercise leads to moderate
changes in global self-esteem. Some light can be shed on why these findings differ
if considered in terms of multidimensional or hierarchical models of self-esteem
discussed earlier (e.g., Sonstroem & Morgan, 1989). In this regard it has been
noted that the smallest change in self-esteem occurs at the global level. Part of the
reason for this is that exercise participation is thought to have the greatest influ-
ence on self-esteem at domain-specific levels (e.g., physical self-worth, physical
competence).
While McDonald and Hodgdon (1991) included domain-specific self-
evaluations in their study, these were subsumed into, and used interchangeably
with, a global construct, i.e., self-concept. Given that the current meta-analysis
employed only measures of GSE, it is possible that McDonald and Hodgdon’s
(1991) findings regarding self-esteem and exercise participation were larger because
of their inclusion of these domain-specific components.
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
322 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
It has been claimed that self-concept is the psychological construct with the
most potential for change because of participation in an exercise program (e.g.,
Folkins & Sime, 1981; Hughes, 1984). In comparison with meta-analyses of other
psychological constructs, it appears that state anxiety (dd = –0.34, Long & van
Stavel, 1995; d = –0.25, Petruzzello, Landers, Hatfield, Kubitz, & Salazar, 1991)
and depression (dd = –0.72, Craft & Landers, 1998; d = –0.59, North et al., 1990)
show just as much if not more change than GSE as a result of exercise. Thus, from
the results of the present meta-analysis it appears that the benefits of exercise for
GSE are overstated in the literature on physical activity and exercise. Much of this
optimism about the potential effectiveness for exercise increasing GSE is probably
the result of the McDonald and Hodgdon (1991) review. Also, earlier reviews (e.g.,
Folkins & Sime, 1981) base their claims about the benefits of exercise on the results
of a few studies, some with unique populations (e.g., mentally retarded students).
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 323
Methodological Issues
No moderator effects were observed for methodological variables in the lit-
erature on exercise and self-esteem. The fact that there was no difference in effect
size between published and unpublished studies suggests it is not because of a lack
of treatment effect that the unpublished studies remain unpublished. Since 58% of
unpublished effect sizes were derived from studies deemed to be of poor quality,
it is likely the quality of those studies that explains why they remain unpublished.
An interesting trend was observed for study quality in that studies rated as either
fair (dd = 0.27) or good (d = 0.31) had larger effect sizes than those rated as poor (d
= 0.17). While no significant within-class variability was detected for study quality
overall, the effect sizes in the poor category were heterogeneous, suggesting that
some other factor may explain the effect size for that group.
Based on the sample sizes of the studies in this meta-analysis, it can be surmised
that GSE studies are generally undersized and underpowered. That is, researchers
are not using samples of sufficient size to provide adequate power in their studies
in order to detect significant findings for the effect of exercise on GSE. This was
the case regardless of whether studies were published or unpublished. In fact, 95%
of the effect sizes in this analysis came from sample sizes too small to detect the
average weighted effect size to be significant. Thus it is likely that lack of statistical
power has contributed to some of the confusion surrounding exercise and GSE.
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
324 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
References
Note: References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.
*Alexander, M.J.L., Nickel, R., Boreskie, S.L., & Searle, M. (2000). Comparison of the
effects of two types of fitness/flexibility programs on gait, mobility and self-esteem
of older females. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 38, 235-268.
*Alfermann, D., & Stoll, O. (2000). Effects of physical exercise on self-concept and well-
being. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 30, 47-65.
Arent, S.M., Landers, D.M., & Etnier, J.L. (2000). The effects of exercise on mood in older
adults: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 8, 407-430.
*Asci, F.H. (2003). The effects of physical fitness training on trait anxiety and physical self-
concept of female university students. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4, 255-264.
Baranowski, T., Anderson, C., & Carmack, C. (1998). Mediating variable framework in
physical activity interventions: How are we doing? How might we do better? American
Journal of Preventive Medicine, 15, 266-297.
*Barker, W.T. (1985). The effect of weight training on self-concept and locus of control in
male patients in treatment for chemical dependence. Unpublished doctoral disserta-
tion, University of San Francisco.
*Bartlett, S.J. (1996). The psychological effects of exercise on dieting obese women (Doctoral
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 325
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
326 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
*Case, S.C. (2003). The effects of group therapy and exercise on depression and self-esteem
in college students. Dissertation Abstracts International, 64(4), B1893. (UMI No.
3086935)
Caspersen, C.J., Powell, K.E., & Christenson, G.M. (1985). Physical activity, exercise, and
physical fitness. Public Health Reports, 100, 125-131.
*Cerrato, M.R. (1977). Changes in body image and self concept among college students
who participate in a skilled program of physical conditioning (Doctoral dissertation,
East Texas State University, 1976). DAI, 37, A4210.
*Clower, M.A. (1979). The relationships of self-concept, movement concept and physical
fitness for college women (Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia, 1978). DAI,
39, A4120.
*Cocklin, J.C. (1989). The effects of physical fitness and body cathexis on self-concept
change in women after aerobic conditioning (Doctoral dissertation, Oklahoma State
University, 1988). DAI, 49, B4594.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.
Colditz, G.A. (1999). Economic costs of obesity and inactivity. Medicine and Science in
Sports and Exercise, 31(11, Suppl.), S663-S667.
*Collingwood, T.R. (1972). The effects of physical training upon behavior and self attitudes.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28, 583-585.
Cooley, C.H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Scribner’s.
*Coyle, C.P., & Santiago, M.C. (1995). Aerobic exercise training and depressive symp-
tomatology in adults with physical disabilities. Archives of Physical Medicine and
Rehabilitation, 76, 647-652.
Craft, L.L., & Landers, D.M. (1998). The effect of exercise on clinical depression and
depression resulting from mental illness: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology, 20, 339-357.
*Cusumano, J.A., & Robinson, S.E. (1992). The short-term psychophysiological effects of
Hatha yoga and progressive relaxation on female Japanese students. Applied Psychol-
ogy: An International Review, 42, 77-90.
*Davis, M.W. (1971). The effects of a cardiovascular conditioning program on selected
psychological responses of college males (Doctoral dissertation, University of Okla-
homa, 1970). DAI, 32, A221.
*de Geus, E.J., van Doornen, L.J.P., & Orlebeke, J.F. (1993). Regular exercise and aerobic
fitness in relation to psychological make-up and physiological stress reactivity. Psy-
chosomatic Medicine, 55, 347-363.
*de Piano, F.A., de Piano, L.C., Carter, W., & Wanlass, R.L. (1984). Physical fitness training:
Adjunctive treatment for the depressed, low self-esteem and muscular tense patient.
Psychotherapy in Private Practice, 2(3), 75-83.
*Dewlen, R.F. (1991). Self-concept changes in physically disabled adult men following
eight weeks of physical fitness training (Doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas,
1990). DAI, 52, A465.
*Dickman, N., & Shea, J. (2002). Does exercise or the exercise setting affect adult self-con-
cept? In Abstract Book of the 12th Commonwealth International Sport Conference
(p. 159). London: Association of Commonwealth Universities.
*DiLorenzo, T.M., Bargman, E.P., Stucky-Ropp, R., Brassington, G.S., Frensch, P.A., &
LaFontaine, T. (1999). Long-term effects of aerobic exercise on psychological out-
comes. Preventive Medicine, 28, 75-85.
Dishman, R.K. (1994). Prescribing exercise intensity for healthy adults using perceived
exertion. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 26, 1087-1094.
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 327
*Donaldson, C.S. (1979). The effects of self-defense training on women’s self-esteem and
locus of control orientation (Doctoral dissertation, California School of Professional
Psychology, 1978). DAI, 39, B3505.
*Dreher, E.R. (1974). The effects of hatha yoga and judo on personality and self-concept
profiles on college men and women (Doctoral dissertation, University of Utah, 1973).
DAI, 34, A4833.
Dunlap, W.P., Cortina, J.M., Vaslow, J.B., & Burke, M.J. (1996). Meta-analysis of experiments with
matched groups or repeated measure designs. Psychological Methods, 1, 170-177.
*Edlund, C.J. (1972). Physiological, anthropometrical, and self-concept changes in over-
weight college women as affected by exercise and voluntary diet. Unpublished master’s
thesis, South Dakota State University.
*Eickhoff, J., Thorland, W., & Ansorge, C. (1983). Selected physiological and psychological
effects of aerobic dancing among young adult women. Journal of Sports Medicine
and Physical Fitness, 23, 273-280.
Field, A. (2001). Meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: A Monte Carlo comparison of
fixed- and random-effects methods. Psychological Methods, 6, 161-180.
Folkins, C.H., & Sime, W.E. (1981). Physical fitness training and mental health. American
Psychologist, 36, 373-389.
*Follansbee, P.A. (1983). Effects of a self-defense program on women’s psychological
health and well-being (Doctoral dissertation, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale,
1982). DAI, 43, B2388.
*Ford, H.T., Puckett, J.R., Blessing, D.L., & Tucker, L.A. (1989). Effects of selected physi-
cal activities on health-related fitness and psychological well-being. Psychological
Reports, 64, 203-208.
*Ford, H.T., Puckett, J.R., Reeve, T.G., & Lefavi, R.G. (1991). Effects of selected physical
activities on global self-concept and body-cathexis scores. Psychological Reports, 68,
1339-1343.
Fox, K.R. (1997). The physical self: From motivation to well-being. Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Fox, K.R. (2000). Self-esteem, self-perceptions and exercise. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 31, 228-240.
Fox, K.R., & Corbin, C.B. (1989). The Physical Self-Perception Profile: Development and
preliminary validation. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 11, 408-430.
*Fulton, G.B. (1968). Study of the relationship of body image to selected physical abilities:
A phenomenonological approach to analysis and modification (Doctoral dissertation,
University of Maryland, 1967). DAI, 28, A3486.
*Garofoli, E. (1990). The influence of a walking program on the body cathexis and self-esteem
of adult sedentary women. Unpublished master’s thesis, Springfield College.
*Gary, V., & Guthrie, D. (1972). The effect of jogging on physical fitness and self-concept
in hospitalized alcoholics. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 33, 1073-1078.
Gauvin, L., Spence, J.C., & Anderson, S. (1999). Exercise and psychological well-being in
the adult population: Reality or wishful thinking? In J.M. Rippe (Ed.), Textbook of
medicine, exercise, nutrition and health (pp. 957-966). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
*Gibbs, T.S. (1987). The effects of a geriatric exercise program on self-concept. Unpublished
master’s thesis, Brigham Young University.
*Gillis, A.J., & Perry, A. (1991). The relationships between physical activity and health-pro-
moting behaviours in mid-life women. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 16, 299-310.
Glass, G.V., McGaw, B., & Smith, M.L. (1981). Meta-analysis in social research. Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
328 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
*Glassford, J.M. (1993). The effect of exercise on physical fitness, physical self-efficacy,
physical competence, physical acceptance, self-esteem, locus of control, and hope
(Master’s thesis, University of Alberta, 1992). MAI, 31, 995.
*Goldberg, W.G., & Fitzpatrick, J.J. (1980). Movement therapy with the aged. Nursing
Research, 29, 339-346.
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Rosenblatt, A., Burling, J., Lyon, D., Simon,
L., & Pinel, E. (1992). Why do people need self-esteem? Converging evidence that
self-esteem serves an anxiety-buffering function. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63, 913-922.
Gruber, J.J. (1986). Physical activity and self-esteem development in children: A meta-
analysis. In G.A. Stull & H.M. Eckert (Eds.), Effects of physical activity on children
(pp. 30-48). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
*Gruen, C.T. (1991). The effect of a walking program upon self-concept in male patients in
treatment for chemical dependency (Doctoral dissertation, University of San Francisco,
1990). DAI, 52, A115.
*Hannaford, C.P. (1984). The psychophysiological effects of a running program on depres-
sion, self-esteem, and anxiety (Doctoral dissertation, North Texas State University,
1983). DAI, 44, B3527.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York:
Guilford Press.
Hattie, J. (1992). Self-concept. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
*Hayden, R.M., Allen, G.J., & Camaione, D.N. (1986). Some psychological benefits resulting
from involvement in an aerobic fitness from the perspectives of participants and knowl-
edgeable informants. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 26, 67-76.
Heaps, R.A. (1978). Relating physical and psychological fitness: A psychological point of
view. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 18, 399-408.
Hedges, L.V. (1994). Fixed effects models. In H. Cooper & L.V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook
of research synthesis (pp. 285-299). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Hedges, L.V., & Olkin, I. (Eds.) (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando,
FL: Academic Press.
Hedges, L.V., & Vevea, J.L. (1998). Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis.
Psychological Methods, 3, 486-504.
*Henderson, J.M. (1974). The effect of physical conditioning on self-concept in college females
(Doctoral dissertation, Washington State University, 1974). DAI, 35, B3063.
*Henning, J.M. (1987). The effect of a regular exercise program on the self-concept and self-
actualization of geriatric persons (Doctoral dissertation, United States International
University, 1987). DAI, 48, B1800.
*Hilyer, J.C., & Mitchell, J.W. (1979). Effect of systematic physical fitness training com-
bined with counseling on the self-concept of college students. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 26, 427-436.
*Horowitz, S.M. (1985). Job stress: Correlates, differences, and intervention effects on health,
attitude and lifestyle behavior in university faculty and administrators (Doctoral dis-
sertation, The University of Michigan, 1984). DAI, 45, A3577.
*Howse, R. (2002). The effects of a resistance training program on selected psychological vari-
ables in sedentary females (Master’s thesis, University of Alberta, 1986). MAI, 40.
Hughes, J.R. (1984). Psychological effects of habitual aerobic exercise: A critical review.
Preventive Medicine, 13, 66-78.
*Hurley, M.F. (1971). The effects of basic swimming instruction upon self-concept. Unpub-
lished master’s thesis, University of Montana.
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 329
James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover. (Original work publ.
1890)
*Jette, N.M. (1976). The effect of modern dance and music on body image and self concept in
college women (Master’s thesis, Brigham Young University, 1975). DAI, 37, A178.
*Jones, A.T. (1985). The effects of jazz dance and aerobic dance on self-concept. Completed
Research in Health, Physical Education, Recreation, and Dance, 27, 598.
*Jones, M.R. (1988). The effect of participation in a wellness program on cardiovascular
function and self-concept in middle aged men and women. Unpublished master’s
thesis, South Dakota State University.
Katzmarzyk, P.T., Gledhill, N., & Shephard, R.J. (2000). The economic burden of physical
inactivity in Canada. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 163, 1435-1440.
*Kelsey, C.W. (1973). Therapeutic recreation and self-concepts of selected psychiatric
patients. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Utah.
*Klein, M.H., Greist, J.H., Gurman, A.S., Neimeyer, R.A., Lesser, D.P., Bushnell, N.J., &
Smith, R.E. (1985). A comparative outcome study of group-psychotherapy vs exercise
treatments for depression. International Journal of Mental Health, 13, 148-177.
*Knittle, V.R. (1988). The effect of twelve weeks of exercise on depression, self-esteem, and
locus-of-control in a wellness program sample of women aged 65 and older (Master’s
thesis, University of Arizona). MAI, 26, 380.
*Kofsky, R.D. (1983). Skill mastery and self-esteem: A comparison of physical condition-
ing with other skills (Doctoral dissertation, Ball State University, 1982). DAI, 43(7),
B2317-B2318. (UMI No. 8227594)
Kraemer, H.C., Stice, E., Kazdin, A., Offord, D., & Kupfer, D. (2001). How do risk factors
work together? Mediators, moderators, and independent, overlapping, and proxy risk
factors. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 848-856.
*Kulinovich, B. (1983). The effect of exercise on the self-concept of physically disabled
elderly (Master’s thesis, University of Arizona, 1982). MAI, 21, 77.
Landers, D.M., & Arent, S.M. (2001). Physical activity and mental health. In R.N. Singer,
H.A. Hausenblas, & C.M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (2nd ed., pp.
740-765). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
*Lauffer, P.L. (1988). The effect of gender, gender role, and weight training on self-concept
and body cathexis (Master’s thesis, University of Arizona, 1987). MAI, 26(1), 24.
(UMI No. 1331411)
*Lee, J.M. (1989). An investigation of the effects of selected physical education activities
upon self-concept of college students (Doctoral dissertation, East Texas State Uni-
versity, 1988). DAI, 49(10), A2963. (UMI No. 8827268)
*Li, F., Harmer, P., Chaumeton, N.R., Duncan, T.E., & Duncan, S.C. (2002). Tai Chi as
a means to enhance self-esteem: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Applied
Gerontology, 21, 70-89.
Long, B.C., & van Stavel, R. (1995). Effects of exercise training on anxiety: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 7, 167-189.
Lou, Y., Abrami, P.C., Spence, J.C., Poulsen, C., Chambers, B., & d’Apollonia, S. (1996).
Within-class grouping: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 66, 423-458.
*Lu, F.J.H. (2003). Effects of 10 weeks’ weight training on female college students’ physical
self and mood. Bulletin of Sport and Exercise Psychology of Taiwan, 3, 111-125.
Marsh, H.W. (1997). The measurement of physical self-concept: A construct validation
approach. In K.R. Fox (Ed.), The physical self: From motivation to well-being (pp.
27-58). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
330 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
Martin, K.A., & Sinden, A.R. (2001). Who will stay and who will go? A review of older
adults’ adherence to randomized controlled trials of exercise. Journal of Aging and
Physical Activity, 9, 91-114.
*May, B.E. (1979). Effects of cardiovascular training on self-concept. Completed Research
in Health, Physical Education, and Recreation, 21, 40.
McArdle, W.D., Katch, F.I., & Katch, V.L. (1986). Exercise physiology: Energy, nutrition,
and human performance (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
McAuley, E., Blissmer, B., Katula, J., Duncan, T.E., & Mihalko, S.L. (2000). Physical
activity, self-esteem, and self-efficacy relationships in older adults: A randomized
controlled trial. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 131-139.
McAuley, E., Mihalko, S.L., & Bane, S.M. (1997). Exercise and self-esteem in middle-aged
adults: Multidimensional relationships and physical-fitness and self-efficacy influences.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 20, 67-83.
*McAuley, J.W., Long, L., Heise, J., Kirby, T., Buckworth, J., Pitt, C., Lehman, K.J., Moore,
J.L., & Reeves, A.L. (2001). A prospective evaluation of the effects of a 12-week
outpatient exercise program on clinical and behavioral outcomes in patients with
epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 2, 592-600.
*McConnell, J.A. (1989). Effects of movement training on body awareness, self-concept,
and antisocial behavior in forensic psychiatric patients (Doctoral dissertation, The
Ohio State University, 1988). DAI, 49(9), B4066. (UMI No. 8824568)
McDonald, D.G., & Hodgdon, J.A. (1991). Psychological effects of aerobic fitness training.
New York: Springer.
Mead, G.H. (1934). Mind, self, and society from the standpoint of a social behaviorist.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
*Melnick, M.J., & Mookerjee, S. (1991). Effects of advanced weight training on body-cathexis
and self-esteem. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 1335-1345.
*Metcalfe, L.L. (1980). The effect of a ten week parcours exercise program on self-concept,
body-cathexis, life and job satisfaction of state employees. Unpublished master’s
thesis, Florida State University.
*Millett, H.L. (1974). An evaluation of social and physical changes in social dance stu-
dents at Brigham Young University. Unpublished master’s thesis, Brigham Young
University.
*Mock, V., Burke, M.B., Sheehan, P., Creator, E.M., Winningham, M.L., McKenney-Tedder,
S., Schwager, L.P., & Liebman, M. (1994). A nursing rehabilitation program for
women with breast cancer receiving adjuvant chemotherapy. Oncology Nursing
Forum, 21, 899-907.
*Moore, K.A. (1995). The effect of participation in an aerobic exercise program on the
self-esteem of older adults (Doctoral dissertation, Kent State University, 1995). DAI,
56(6), B3429. (UMI No. 9536673)
Morgan, W.P. (1985). Affective beneficence of vigorous physical activity. Medicine and
Science in Sports and Exercise, 17, 94-101.
*Muenchow, J.B. (1990). The effect of exercise and sport activities on the psychological state
and trait tendencies of college-age nonparticipants (Doctoral dissertation, University
of Wyoming, 1990). DAI, 51(6), A1951. (UMI No. 9030617)
*Muller, S.M. (1993). The effects of exercise-induced changes on self-esteem (Doctoral
dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park, 1993). DAI, 54, A1688.
North, T.C., McCullagh, P., & Tran, Z.V. (1990). Effect of exercise on depression. Exercise
and Sport Sciences Reviews, 18, 379-415.
*Norvell, N., Martin, D., & Salmon, A. (1991). Psychological and physiological benefits of
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 331
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
332 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
Exercise and Self-Esteem / 333
Sonstroem, R.J. (1997b). The physical self-system: A mediator of exercise and self-esteem.
In K.R. Fox (Ed.), The physical self: From motivation to well-being (pp. 3-26).
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Sonstroem, R.J. (1998). Physical self-concept: Assessment and external validity. Exercise
and Sport Sciences Reviews, 28, 133-164.
Sonstroem, R.J., & Morgan, W.P. (1989). Exercise and self-esteem: Rationale and model.
Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 21, 329-337.
*Sorensen, M., Anderssen, S., Hjerman, I., Holme, I., & Ursin, H. (1997). Exercise and diet
interventions improve perceptions of self in middle-aged adults. Scandinavian Journal
of Medicine & Science in Sports, 7, 312-320.
Spence, J.C., & Blanchard, C. (2001). Publication bias in sport and exercise psychology
research: The games we play. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 32, 386-
399.
*Stefani, U. (2002). The effects of a physical conditioning program on the physical fitness and
self-concept of elderly women (Master’s thesis, McGill University, 1988). MAI, 40.
*Stein, P.N., & Motta, R.W. (1992). Effects of aerobic and nonaerobic exercise on depres-
sion and self-concept. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74, 79-89.
*Stewart, A.L., Mills, K.M., Sepsis, P.G., King, A.C., Mclellan, B.Y., Roitz, K., & Ritter,
P.L. (1997). Evaluation of CHAMPS, a physical activity promotion program for older
adults. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 19, 353-361.
*Teng, L.N. (1982). The relationship of self-esteem to satisfaction with social supports,
self-report of socially valued resources, and interpersonal reciprocity before and after
participation in a systematic aerobic dance exercise program (Doctoral dissertation,
University of Washington, 1982). DAI, 43(6), B2008. (UMI No. 8226608)
*Titzel, M.R. (1986). The effects of a dance-movement program on the self-concept of
institutionalized, intermediate care, female elderly. Unpublished master’s thesis, The
Pennsylvania State University.
*Tseo, C. (1983). The effect of an aerobic dance program on the self-concepts and physi-
cal fitness levels of young adult institutionalized female mental patients. Completed
Research in Health, Physical Education, Recreation, and Dance, 25, 178.
*Tucker, L.A. (1982). Effect of weight-training program on the self-concepts of college
males. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 1055-1061.
*Tucker, L.A. (1983). Weight training: A tool for the improvement of self and body concepts
of males. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 9, 31-37.
U.S. Dept. of Health and Human Services. (1996). Physical activity and health: A report
of the Surgeon General. Atlanta: U.S. Dept. HHS, Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion.
*Weinstein, J.B. (1986). The influences of physical fitness on mood and self-esteem in a
young adult population: A learned efficacy model (Doctoral dissertation, State Uni-
versity of New York at Albany, 1986). DAI, 47, B2637-2638.
*Wescott, R.P. (1980). The effect of elective physical education classes on the self-concept of
college students (Doctoral dissertation, Oklahoma State U., 1980). DAI, 41, A3482.
*White, A.J. (1974). The interrelationships between measures of physical fitness and mea-
sures of self-concept of selected Mississippi State University male students (Doctoral
dissertation, Mississippi State University, 1973). DAI, 34, A4849.
*Whiting, J.W. (1981). The effect of a mild exercise program on the psychological treatment
of inpatient alcoholics (Doctoral dissertation, Fuller Theological Seminary, School
of Psychology, 1980). DAI, 42, B394-B395.
*Williams, D.D. (1972). Instruction stressing physical fitness compared with instruction
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC
334 / Spence, McGannon, and Poon
stressing skill acquisition upon self-concept in Caucasian and Negro students (Doctoral
dissertation, University of Arkansas, 1972). DAI, 33, A2156.
*Yeagle, G.W. (1982). The effect of a physical recreation program on the self-concept and
flexibility of senior citizens (Doctoral dissertation, University of Utah, 1981). DAI,
42, B3184.
Notes
1 When exploring the self in the physical domain, the terms self-esteem and self-con-
cept tend to be used interchangeably; there is no one unified, operational definition per se.
McDonald and Hodgdon (1991) used the two terms in a unified form, and for the purposes
in this paper so do we.
2 According to Caspersen et al. (1985), physical fitness consists of both health related
(n = 80) of the effect sizes came from unpublished sources. This is a much greater proportion
of unpublished effect sizes than is typically seen in meta-analyses. In fact, the majority of
meta-analyses in sport and exercise psychology include no unpublished works (see Gauvin
et al., 1999).
4 Rosenthal (1995) does not agree that moderator analyses should only proceed after
the detection of significant heterogeneity of variance among a set of effect sizes. He argues
that scientific progress can result from scientists “continually reducing the magnitude of
sampling error by increasing their understanding of moderator variables” (p. 186). While
Overton (1998) claims such exploratory analyses are justified in meta-analyses as long as
they are preceded by homogeneity testing and conducted on a priori hypothesized modera-
tors, Rosenthal states that “planned contrasts should be computed without reference to the
overall F, and even unplanned contrasts can be computed with appropriate adjustments of
their levels of significance” (p. 188).
5 Lifestyle programs included exercise along with stress reduction and nutrition
counseling. Skills training activities were those in which the goal was primarily to develop
competence in a skill as opposed to enhancing physical fitness. Such activities included
martial arts and physical education courses.
6 One reviewer of this paper inquired about the statistical power of our analysis.
According to Field (2001), we have sufficient power, based both on number of studies (k
= 118) and average sample size (n = 61), at an overall level to detect any significant effect
sizes. True, we may be underpowered in several levels of our moderator variables. However,
none of the effect sizes for these levels is greater than d = .15. Thus we do not think we are
missing any substantial effects in those categories due to low power.
Acknowledgment
We express our appreciation to Trish Dyck who provided assistance with the study.
Brought to you by ULB - TEAM HUMANWISSENSCHAFTEN | Unauthenticated | Downloaded 12/01/24 02:42 PM UTC