Brain Imaging and Behavior (2008) 2:192–199
DOI 10.1007/s11682-008-9025-4
Neural Correlates of Visuospatial Working Memory
in Healthy Young Adults at Risk for Hypertension
Andreana P. Haley & John Gunstad & Ronald A. Cohen &
Beth A. Jerskey & Richard C. Mulligan &
Lawrence H. Sweet
Received: 1 October 2007 / Accepted: 6 June 2008 / Published online: 27 June 2008
# Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2008
Abstract Family history of hypertension (FH+) has been
associated with subtle deficits in cognitive function. In
search of an early marker that may identify individuals
predisposed to developing cognitive difficulties, we
employed fMRI to test for FH+ related differences in
hemodynamic response to a working memory challenge in
healthy young adults with intact working memory. Fourteen
healthy adults (ages 18 to 40 years) participated in an fMRI
study of working memory. Seven of the participants were
FH+. Groups were matched for working memory performance. Relative to FH− controls, FH+ participants
exhibited lower 2-Back-related activation in the right
inferior parietal lobule and the right inferior temporal gyrus
as well as significantly more deactivation in the posterior
cingulate cortex. These results indicate that FH+ is
associated with subtle changes in visuospatial attention
even in healthy young adults.
Keywords Hypertension . Attention . fMRI . Cognition .
Genetic risk . Family history
A. P. Haley (*)
Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin,
1 University Station, A8000,
Austin, TX 78712, USA
e-mail: haley@psy.utexas.edu
J. Gunstad
Department of Psychology, Kent State University,
Kent, OH, USA
R. A. Cohen : B. A. Jerskey : R. C. Mulligan : L. H. Sweet
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior,
Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University,
Providence, RI, USA
Introduction
Hypertension (HTN), defined as systolic blood pressure
greater than 140 mmHg or diastolic blood pressure
greater than 90 mmHg (American Heart Association
2004), is a common human disease associated with
significant morbidity and mortality worldwide (Dickson
and Sigmund 2006). HTN is the most common risk factor
for multiple serious medical conditions such as coronary
heart disease (Clarke et al. 2002; Lawes et al. 2002;
Lewington et al. 2002) and stroke (Sacco et al. 1999;
Gorelick 2003; Dennis et al. 1989). HTN is also a known
risk factor for poor neurocognitive outcome, including
cerebral white matter disease (Hassan et al. 2003; O’Brien
et al. 2003; Pantoni and Garcia 1997) and generalized
cerebral atrophy (van Dijk et al. 2004; Farkas and Luiten
2001). Many HTN patients also develop serious cognitive
problems later in life (Farkas and Luiten 2001; Lindsay et
al. 1997; Prince et al. 1994). While cognitive sequelae
have historically received less attention than the cardiac and
cerebrovascular effects of HTN, there is accumulating
evidence that cognitive functioning is an important determinant of health status, quality of life, and functional ability
(Ades et al. 1992; Cohen et al. 1999). These findings provide
a compelling reason for identifying early markers of risk for
cognitive impairment among individuals with current HTN
as well as those at risk for HTN in order to improve
prevention of dementia, and ensure early treatment.
Positive family history of HTN (FH+), defined as
HTN before age 60 in a first-degree relative, is a
powerful risk factor for future development of the
disease. The risk increases progressively as a function
of the number of affected parents and younger parental
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2008) 2:192–199
age of onset (Hunt and Williams 1994). FH+ has now
become an accepted surrogate marker of genetic risk for
HTN (Kailasam et al. 2000; Lacy et al. 1998; O’Connor et
al. 2002; Schneider et al. 2003; Song et al. 2000). FH+ is
associated with pre-clinical pathological changes in a
number of important physiological domains (Brinton et
al. 1996; Grunfeld et al. 1990; Kailasam et al. 2000; Lacy
et al. 1998; Li et al. 2005; O’Connor et al. 2002; Schneider
et al. 2003; Song et al. 2000; Zhang et al. 2004) as well as
diminished performance on neuropsychological tests
(Thyrum et al. 1995; Waldstein et al. 1994; Ditto et al.
2006). Affected cognitive domains include visuospatial/
constructional ability, verbal learning, attention, and shortterm memory. These cognitive findings have been found
to be independent of psychological state and health (e.g.,
anxiety, depression) as well as other commonly found
confounds such as age, gender, ethnicity, education, and
current blood pressure (Thyrum et al. 1995; Waldstein
et al. 1994). Therefore, FH+ appears to identify a subgroup of individuals at especially high risk for cognitive
impairment.
In this study, we employed functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) to determine how the pattern
of brain activity during a working memory task may
vary as a function of family history of HTN in healthy
young adults with intact working memory performance.
We chose a working memory task because it taxes
attention and short-term memory, both known to vary as
a function of family history of HTN (Thyrum et al.
1995; Waldstein et al. 1994). Based on previous studies
indicating that FH+ individuals exhibit vascular alterations
(e.g., mild endothelial dysfunction) at a very early age (Li
et al. 2005), we hypothesized that positive family history
of HTN would be associated with lower BOLD response to
a cognitive challenge due to mild cerebral hypoperfusion.
Our hypothesis extended previous literature documenting
that poor peripheral perfusion is related to diminished
cerebrovascular health and impaired cognitive function in
patients with cardiovascular disease (Gunstad et al. 2005;
Haley et al. 2007a, 2007b; Hoth et al. 2007; Jefferson et
al. 2007a, 2007b; Paul et al. 2005). We hypothesized that
sub-clinical abnormalities in the vasculature of FH+
individuals will result in lower, yet sufficient, levels of
oxygenated hemoglobin being delivered to active brain
areas and lower BOLD response to a cognitive challenge
at normal levels of behavioral performance. Our confidence in the sensitivity of fMRI was based on a growing
body of clinical research literature indicating that fMRI
can detect altered brain function among individuals at risk
before clinical symptoms or behavioral deficits are present
(Bookheimer et al. 2000; Chang et al. 2001; Sweet et al.
2006).
193
Materials and methods
Participants
Fourteen right-handed participants (9 women and 5 men)
between the ages of 18 and 40 were recruited through flyers
posted in the Providence, RI area.
Family history of hypertension (FH+) Participants with
positive family history of HTN were especially solicited
during recruitment. Upon enrollment in the study, participants’ family history of HTN was confirmed using the
Ohio Blood Pressure History Survey, a brief instrument
used to document family history of HTN in past studies
(France et al. 2000). The overall accuracy of the measure as
compared to medical records has been documented at
94.2% (Page and France 2001). The participants’ parents
completed the survey. Parental medical records were not
solicited. The sensitivity and specificity of the Ohio Blood
Pressure History Survey have been noted at 95.4% and
92.4%, respectively (Page and France 2001). Consistent
with previous studies, positive family history of HTN (FH+)
was defined as HTN before age 60 in a first-degree relative
(parent) (Kailasam et al. 2000; Lacy et al. 1998; Schneider
et al. 2003; Song et al. 2000). Included in the FH− group
were participants with two normotensive parents. The final
sample included seven FH− and seven FH+ normotensive
young adults. Both groups were mixed in terms of sex, and
balanced in terms of age, education, and weight. Details
about the demographics of the two groups are available in
Table 1.
Participants were excluded from participation if they
had a history of neurological disease (i.e., large vessel
stroke, seizure disorder, Parkinson’s disease, clinically
significant traumatic brain injury, multiple sclerosis, or
brain infection/meningitis), major psychiatric illness (e.g.
schizophrenia, bipolar disorder), substance abuse (i.e.,
diagnosed abuse and/or previous hospitalization for substance abuse), diagnosed current hypertension, or MRI
contraindications.
Table 1 Demographic information
Number of
patients
Sex (female/
male)
Age
Education
Weight
FH+ Group
FH− Group
7
7
4/3
5/2
27.14 (3.53)
18.14 (3.67)
147.57 (34.49)
29.00 (6.32)
18.29 (1.80)
147.43 (32.71)
t
p value
−0.678
−0.092
0.008
0.510
0.928
0.994
194
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2008) 2:192–199
Working memory paradigms
Working memory was assessed using visuospatial and verbal
n-Back tasks (Awh et al. 1996; Braver et al. 1997; Smith and
Jonides 1997). During the visuospatial n-Back task, a series
of black dots was visually presented for 500 ms each with a
2500 ms inter-stimulus interval (Fig. 1). The location of the
black dot changed with each presentation. Participants were
asked to determine if the location of each dot was the same
as, or different from, a previously presented stimulus. During
the verbal n-Back, consonants were presented at the same
rate in the center of the screen. For each stimulus participants
were asked to determine if the consonant was the same as, or
different from, a previously presented consonant. Responses
were collected using a two-button response box. A right
index finger response indicated affirmative and a right
middle finger response indicated negative. In both the
visuospatial and verbal paradigms a 0-Back control condition
was alternated with the 2-Back condition in a block design.
An imaging run consisted of four blocks of the 0-Back and
four blocks of the 2-Back lasting approximately 6 min.
Visuospatial processing was the focus of the current
investigation. The visuospatial 2-Back has been used in
previous studies involving both patient populations and
healthy participants (Awh et al. 1996; Smith et al. 2006;
Chang et al. 2004; Kwon et al. 2001). Visuospatial perception, short-term memory buffering, attention, and executive
coordination are all required for successful task performance.
0-Back control condition This task consisted of four blocks
of nine visual stimuli in random order, 33% of which were
targets. Participants responded “yes” when a dot appeared
in the center of the screen, and “no” if a dot appeared in any
other location.
2-Back working memory condition The experimental condition consisted of four blocks, each containing 15 visual
stimuli in random order, 33% of which were targets. A
stimulus was considered a target if it was in the same
location as the stimulus presented two stimuli earlier.
Procedures
The local Institutional Review Board approved the study
and all volunteers provided written informed consent before
enrollment. Participants completed a medical history interview with one of the team members (LS or JG).
Cardiovascular risk factors such as hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, tobacco use, and diabetes were coded as
either present or absent according to participants’ selfreport. Handedness was recorded according to participants’
stated hand preference and confirmed by observing a
writing sample. Family history of HTN was assessed as
described above.
Each imaging session included at least four blocks of
visuospatial 0-Back/2-Back practice, at least four blocks of
verbal 0-Back/2-Back practice, one imaging run of the
visuospatial 2-Back task, one imaging run of the verbal 2Back task, and T1-weighted imaging for anatomical reference. If necessary, more practice blocks were administered.
All participants practiced the task until they performed
above chance levels. The 2-Back task was presented using
E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA), back-projected onto a screen positioned at the
participant’s feet, and viewed through a double-mirror
attached to the head coil. Participants’ responses were
collected using an MR-compatible piano-key response box.
MRI data acquisition
MRI data for each participant were acquired in a single
session on a 1.5T Siemens Symphony scanner equipped
with a standard head coil. Functional imaging was
Fig. 1 0-Back and 2-Back
visuospatial working memory
task example stimuli
Target
Target
VS 2-Back Task:
Is the dot in the same
location as the dot
shown two before?
Target
Targett
Target
Target
0-Back Task:
Is the dot in the
center of the
screen?
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2008) 2:192–199
195
performed using a whole brain echo-planer imaging (EPI)
sequence (TR=3860 ms, TE=38 ms, FOV=192 mm2, 64×
64 matrix, 48 axial slices, 3 mm slice thickness). Structural
imaging sequences included a high-resolution (256×256
matrix, FOV=256 mm2, 1 mm slice thickness) magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MP-RAGE) anatomical
scan of the entire brain in the saggital plane.
two groups were compared on working memory performance and mean 2-Back related activation intensity within
each empirically defined ROI using independent samples ttests. Data were analyzed using SPSS 11.0 computer
software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). A two-tailed alpha level
of 0.05 was used as the criterion for statistical significance.
fMRI data preprocessing
Results
All EPI images were processed using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software (Cox 1996). Each
time series was spatially registered to the sixth volume of
the session to reduce the effects of head movement. This
AFNI 3-dimensional registration program also yields
information on displacement and rotation for each volume
that was used later to further correct motion. Data preprocessing also included temporal smoothing, spatial
filtering, and transformation to standard stereotaxic space
(Talairach and Tournoux 1988). Task-related brain activation was determined using voxel-wise multiple regression
analyses with the following parameters: a 0-Back/2-Back
reference waveform convolved with a gamma function, and
covariates accounting for instruction screens, head movement, and linear trends.
An empirically defined set of ROIs (group mask) was
created as follows: First, results from individual multiple
regression analyses were transformed into z scores, corrected for multiple comparisons using the false discovery
rate (FDR) correction supplied by AFNI, and thresholded at
p<0.05. Voxels with task-related activation exceeding this
threshold were included the mask if they were active in
either group of participants. Finally, active voxels were
defined as a cluster if they were contiguous and formed a
volume of at least 300 μL. Four cortical regions of interest
(ROIs) resulted (Table 2, Fig. 2). This empirically defined
mask was applied to individual data to determine mean
task-related activation intensity (corrected p<0.05) within
each ROI.
All variable distributions fulfilled the assumption of
normality (Mean Shapiro-Wilk=0.94, mean p=.52). No
variable transformations were performed.
All participants exhibited intact working memory performance (mean accuracy>60%), which was not surprising
considering that they were over-trained to perform the
tasks. Mean accuracy (SD) on the verbal 2-Back task was
82% (11%) correct responses, and mean reaction time (SD)
was 1071.93 (240.00) ms. The performance of the two
groups was equivalent in both accuracy (t(10)=0.163,
p=.874), and reaction time (t(10)=0.73, p=.484). All
participants moved less than 1.5 mm per imaging run.
Figure 2 represents the areas of significant 2-Back related
activity for at least 90% of our participants. The identified
areas of activity also served as our ROI mask. The areas are
consistent with regions of 2-Back related activity in the
published literature (Braver et al. 1997; Smith et al. 1997;
Sweet et al. 2008). Relative to FH− controls, FH+
participants exhibited lower BOLD response to the 2-Back
task in the right inferior parietal lobule (t(12)=2.42,
p=.033) and the right inferior temporal gyrus (t(12)=2.33,
p=.038) (Fig. 3). Relative to FH− controls, FH+ participants
also exhibited substantially more deactivation in the posterior cingulate cortex, bilaterally (t(12)=4.14, p=.001).
Statistical analyses
All variable distributions were examined using the ShapiroWilk test of normality recommended for small samples. The
Discussion
This study examined the relationship between family
history of HTN and brain response to a working memory
challenge in healthy young adults with intact working
memory performance. We found that family history of
HTN, a marker of increased genetic risk for developing the
disease, was associated with lower task-related activation in
Table 2 Differences in the BOLD response to a visuospatial working memory task according to family history of HTN
ROI
|X|
Y
Z
Region
t
1
2
3
4
55
2
26
48
−44
−55
−68
−49
−11
15
37
41
Right inferior temporal gyrus
Bilateral posterior cingulate
Right precuneus
Right inferior parietal lobule
2.33
4.14
0.49
2.42
p value
0.038
0.001
0.630
0.033
196
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2008) 2:192–199
Fig. 2 Regions of interest on
template anatomy. 1 Right inferior temporal gyrus (green); 2
bilateral posterior cingulate gyrus (red); 3 right precuneus
(yellow); 4 right inferior parietal
lobule (blue)
3
1
2
VS 2-Back Related Activity
the right inferior parietal lobule and right inferior temporal
gyrus as well as substantially greater deactivation of the
posterior cingulate during a visuospatial working memory
task.
Our results fit well with a growing body of clinical research
literature indicating that fMRI is able to detect altered brain
function among individuals at risk, before clinical symptoms
or behavioral deficits are present (Bookheimer et al. 2000;
Chang et al. 2001; Sweet et al. 2006). Unlike those prior
studies, however, the alterations we detected consisted of
lower, rather than higher, BOLD response to a cognitive
challenge in the population at risk. We believe that these
differences are significant, and indicative of a different
underlying mechanism. Our study sample was exclusively
comprised of individuals at increased risk for vascular
cognitive impairment, while other studies have included
populations at risk for cognitive deficits related to multiple
sclerosis (Sweet et al. 2006), HIV (Chang et al. 2001), and
genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease (Bookheimer et al.
2000). Therefore, the mechanisms underlying the observed
changes are likely to be different, e.g., hypoperfusion in our
case vs. inflammatory or other non-vascular processes in the
other cases. Our current findings of lower BOLD response to
a cognitive challenge in participants at risk for HTN with
intact behavioral performance can be explained by the nature
of the BOLD contrast itself. Under normal circumstances,
oxygenated hemoglobin is delivered to areas of increased
neuronal activity in excess of neuronal oxygen consumption,
resulting in a change in the ratio of oxygenated vs.
deoxygenated hemoglobin in the activated brain area and
detectable BOLD signal. We propose that, in the present
case, mild cerebral hypoperfusion related to sub-clinical
1.6
2.0
*
1.4
1.2
pathological vascular changes, results in lower, yet sufficient,
amounts of oxygen being delivered to activated neurons,
thus producing a lower BOLD response to the cognitive
challenge in the at-risk population while maintaining intact
cognitive performance. This proposition is supported by a
growing literature documenting that reduced peripheral
perfusion is related to poor cerebrovascular health and
diminished cognitive function in patients with cardiovascular
disease (Gunstad et al. 2005; Haley et al. 2007a; Hoth et al.
2007; Jefferson et al. 2007a, 2007b; Paul et al. 2005).
Following the same logic, one might hypothesize that as the
vascular problems get worse, and some of the healthy FH+
participants go on to develop clinical cardiovascular disease,
cerebral hypoperfusion will exacerbate, eventually leading to
a decline in cognitive performance. This hypothesis is
supported by results from our lab documenting that in
patients with cardiovascular disease, higher levels of large
vessel atherosclerosis are related to both lower BOLD
response to cognitive challenges and lower levels of
behavioral performance (Haley et al. 2007b).
The task-related deactivations in the posterior cingulate
observed in the FH+ participants in our study are also very
interesting, and in our opinion, consistent with our
reasoning that lower BOLD response to a cognitive
challenge in our population provides evidence of subclinical cognitive impairment. Task-related deactivations in
several brain regions including the posterior cingulate have
been increasingly reported in imaging studies (Binder et al.
1999; Mazoyer et al. 2001; Shulman et al. 1997). These
deactivations, indicating higher neural activity during the
baseline/low-demand conditions as opposed to the experimental/high-demand conditions, appear to be associated
2.5
*
1.5
*
2.0
1.0
1.0
1.5
.5
.8
1.0
0.0
.6
-.5
.4
.2
0.0
-.2
N=
4
.5
-1.0
* p < .05
7
7
FHFH+
Inferior Temporal Gyrus
-1.5
-2.0
N=
0.0
7
7
FHFH+
Posterior Cingulate
-.5
N=
7
7
FHFH+
Inferior Parietal Lobule
Fig. 3 Boxplots of averaged task-related signal intensity within the ROIs demonstrating significantly lower BOLD response to a VS WM task in
healthy young adults with family history of HTN (FH+)
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2008) 2:192–199
with internally generated thoughts involving episodic
memory, planning, problem solving, processing emotions
and self-monitoring (Greicius and Menon 2004; McKiernan
et al. 2006). Task-related deactivations have been shown to
increase in magnitude (i.e., deactivate further) with increasing task difficulty thus giving rise to the hypothesis that
selective withdrawal of attention from self-referential,
stimulus-independent thought in favor of more challenging
cognitive tasks may be a compensatory mechanism
employed by participants who are struggling to complete
the task at hand (McKiernan et al. 2003; Sweet et al. 2008).
Withdrawal of resources from this so-called “default-mode”
brain network responsible for stream of consciousness type
mental images and thoughts (Greicius et al. 2003) has also
been associated with nicotine induced enhancement of
visual spatial attention (Hahn et al. 2007). Therefore, while
in some contexts withdrawal of resources from selfreferential thought may indicate more efficient concentration and better performance, in cases such as ours, where
participants are maintaining normal cognitive performance
in suboptimal conditions (e.g., reduced cerebral perfusion),
this increased withdrawal of resources from irrelevant brain
areas seems to indicate a passive compensatory mechanism
that allows patients at risk to maintain normal cognitive
functioning.
Thus, our results indicate that family history of HTN
may be associated with sub-clinical dysfunction of visuospatial attention, storage and rehearsal requiring increased
effort for successful visuospatial working memory performance necessitating higher levels of concentration in order
to maintain normal levels of behavioral performance. These
results fit well with the idea of a continuum of vascular
related brain dysfunction from the brain-at-risk to vascularrelated cognitive impairments, where disturbances in neural
systems supporting attention are among the earliest signs
(Haley et al. 2007a; Jefferson et al. 2007a; Moser et al.
1999; Paul et al. 2005).
The improved sensitivity of fMRI (over behavioral
performance) to detect sub-clinical cognitive difficulties
may be attributed to our ability to more directly observe
brain function, which minimizes response biases and other
measurement error associated with human behavior. Another
reason is that fMRI allows the simultaneous observation of
the complex and interrelated neural processes that underlie
human behavior. Since observed behavior is the product of
complex interactions among neural networks, direct observation of activity among these networks allows us to
determine not only which brain regions are involved, but
how they interrelate to yield a behavioral product. While
demonstration of decreased behavioral performance and
brain response during cognitive challenges has been a typical
goal in fMRI clinical research, there is now sufficient
empirical support to predict altered brain function among
197
asymptomatic at-risk individuals. Greater brain activity in
expected task-related brain regions, significant recruitment
of unexpected brain regions, and suspension of unrelated
brain activity have each been described among normally
performing at-risk samples (Bookheimer et al. 2000; Chang
et al. 2001; Penner et al. 2003; Staffen et al. 2002; Sweet
et al. 2004, 2006). Therefore, we hypothesized that an
fMRI examination would allow us to detect differences in
the patterns of brain activity in response to a cognitive
challenge among healthy individuals at varying degrees of
risk for HTN and vascular cognitive impairment.
In conclusion, the present study presents promising
preliminary data indicating that genetic history of HTN
may be related to subtle alterations in brain function even in
healthy young adults. The study had a few important
limitations including a relatively small sample size and the
use of self-report to document both family history of HTN,
and current medical status. However, considering the
relatively young age of the participants (average age 28),
and the high rates of agreement between responses to the
Ohio Blood Pressure History Survey and medical records
reported in the literature (~94%, Page and France 2001), it is
unlikely that undetected current HTN in the participants, or
reporting error on behalf of the parents contributed significantly to our results. Soliciting medical records for both
participants and parents in future studies will nonetheless
improve the design and help alleviate any concerns about the
use of self-report. A larger sample size on the other hand will
allow us to extend the present results by examining any
potential gender effects. Future studies may also benefit from
comprehensive evaluations of peripheral cardiovascular
functioning as well as examination of particular gene
polymorphisms. The pathways by which family history of
HTN may affect brain function are also of considerable
interest as they may lead to new ways to treat and prevent the
development of vascular cognitive impairment. Finally, the
sensitivity of this measurement for predicting cognitive
function over time should be investigated further.
Acknowledgements This work was supported by grants from the
Ittleson Foundation (LHS & JG) and the National Institutes of Health
T32AG020498 (APH & BAJ).
References
Ades, P. A., Huang, D., & Weaver, S. O. (1992). Cardiac rehabilitation
participation predicts lower rehospitalization costs. American
Heart Journal, 123, 916–921.
Awh, E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Schumacher, E. H., Koeppe, R. A.,
& Katz, S. (1996). Dissociation of storage and rehearsal in verbal
working memory: evidence from positron emission tomography.
Psychological Science, 7, 25–31.
Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S., Rao, S. M.,
& Cox, R. W. (1999). Conceptual processing during the conscious
198
resting state. A functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 11, 80–95.
Bookheimer, S. Y., Strojwas, M. H., Cohen, M. S., Saunders, A. M.,
Pericak-Vance, M. A., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2000). Patterns of
brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. New
England Journal of Medicine, 343, 450–456.
Braver, T. S., Cohen, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., &
Noll, D. C. (1997). A parametric study of prefrontal cortex
involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage, 5, 49–62.
Brinton, T. J., Kailasam, M. T., Wu, R. A., Cervenka, J. H., Chio, S. S.,
Parmer, R. J., et al. (1996). Arterial compliance by cuff
sphygmomanometer. Application to hypertension and early
changes in subjects at genetic risk. Hypertension, 28, 599–603.
Cardiovascular disease statistics: American Heart Association (2004).
American Heart Association [On-line]. Available: http://www.
americanheart.org
Chang, K., Adleman, N. E., Dienes, K., Simeonova, D. I., Menon, V.,
& Reiss, A. (2004). Anomalous prefrontal-subcortical activation
in familial pediatric bipolar disorder: a functional magnetic
resonance imaging investigation. Archives of General Psychiatry,
61, 781–792.
Chang, L., Speck, O., Miller, E. N., Braun, J., Jovicich, J., Koch, C.,
et al. (2001). Neural correlates of attention and working memory
deficits in HIV patients. Neurology, 57, 1001–1007.
Clarke, R., Lewington, S., Youngman, L., Sherliker, P., Peto, R., &
Collins, R. (2002). Underestimation of the importance of blood
pressure and cholesterol for coronary heart disease mortality in
old age. European Heart Journal, 23, 286–293.
Cohen, R. A., Moser, D. J., Clark, M. M., Aloia, M. S., Cargill, B. R.,
Stefanik, S., et al. (1999). Neurocognitive functioning and
improvement in quality of life following participation in cardiac
rehabilitation. American Journal of Cardiology, 83, 1374–1378.
Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of
functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers in
Biomedical Research, 29, 162–173.
Dennis, M. S., Bamford, J. M., Sandercock, P. A., & Warlow, C. P.
(1989). A comparison of risk factors and prognosis for transient
ischemic attacks and minor ischemic strokes. The Oxfordshire
Community Stroke Project. Stroke, 20, 1494–1499.
Dickson, M. E., & Sigmund, C. D. (2006). Genetic basis of hypertension:
revisiting angiotensinogen. Hypertension, 48, 14–20.
Ditto, B., Seguin, J. R., & Tremblay, R. E. (2006). Neuropsychological characteristics of adolescent boys differing in risk for high
blood pressure. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 31, 231–237.
Farkas, E., & Luiten, P. G. (2001). Cerebral microvascular pathology
in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Progress in Neurobiology, 64,
575–611.
France, C., Suhr, J. A., Fox, L., Gunstad, J., & Spitznagel, B. (2000).
Neuropsychological performance in men and women at risk for
hypertension. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 704–705.
Gorelick, P. B. (2003). Prevention of stroke recurrence. International
Psychogeriatrics, 15(Suppl 1), 167–171.
Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003).
Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of
the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 253–258.
Greicius, M. D., & Menon, V. (2004). Default-mode activity during a
passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting
activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1484–1492.
Grunfeld, B., Perelstein, E., Simsolo, R., Gimenez, M., & Romero, J. C.
(1990). Renal functional reserve and microalbuminuria in offspring of hypertensive parents. Hypertension, 15, 257–261.
Gunstad, J., Cohen, R. A., Tate, D. F., Paul, R. H., Poppas, A., Hoth,
K., et al. (2005). Blood pressure variability and white matter
hyperintensities in older adults with cardiovascular disease.
Blood Pressure, 14(6), 353–358.
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2008) 2:192–199
Hahn, B., Ross, T. J., Yang, Y. H., Kim, I., Huestis, M. A., & Stein, E. A.
(2007). Nicotine enhances visuospatial attention by deactivating
areas of the resting brain default network. Journal of Neuroscience,
27, 3477–3489.
Haley, A. P., Forman, D. E., Poppas, A., Hoth, K. F., Gunstad, J.,
Jefferson, A. L., et al. (2007a). Carotid artery intima-media
thickness and cognition in cardiovascular disease. International
Journal of Cardiology, 121, 148–154.
Haley, A. P., Sweet, L. H., Gunstad, J., Forman, D. E., Poppas, A.,
Paul, R. H., et al. (2007b). Verbal working memory and
atherosclerosis in patients with cardiovascular disease: an fMRI
study. Journal of Neuroimaging, 17(3), 227–233.
Hassan, A., Hunt, B. J., O’Sullivan, M., Parmar, K., Bamford, J. M.,
Briley, D., et al. (2003). Markers of endothelial dysfunction in
lacunar infarction and ischaemic leukoaraiosis. Brain, 126, 424–
432.
Hoth, K. F., Tate, D. F., Poppas, A., Forman, D. E., Gunstad, J.,
Moser, D. J., et al. (2007). Endothelial Function and White
Matter Hyperintensities in Older Adults With Cardiovascular
Disease. Stroke, 38(2), 308–312.
Hunt, S. C., & Williams, R. R. (1994). Genetic factors in human
hypertension. In J. Swales (Ed.), Textbook of Hypertension (pp.
519–538). Oxford: Blackwell.
Jefferson, A. L., Poppas, A., Paul, R. H., & Cohen, R. A. (2007a).
Systemic hypoperfusion is associated with executive dysfunction
in geriatric cardiac patients. Neurobiology of Aging, 28, 477–483.
Jefferson, A. L., Tate, D. F., Poppas, A., Brickman, A. M., Paul, R. H.,
Gunstad, J., et al. (2007b). Lower cardiac output is associated
with greater white matter hyperintensities in older adults with
cardiovascular disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics
Society, 55(7), 1044–1048.
Kailasam, M. T., Parmer, R. J., Tyrell, E. A., Henry, R. R., &
O’Connor, D. T. (2000). Circulating amylin in human essential
hypertension: heritability and early increase in individuals at
genetic risk. Journal de l’hypertension, 18, 1611–1620.
Kwon, H., Menon, V., Eliez, S., Warsofsky, I. S., White, C. D., DyerFriedman, J., et al. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of visuospatial working memory in fragile X syndrome: relation to
behavioral and molecular measures. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 158, 1040–1051.
Lacy, F., O’Connor, D. T., & Schmid-Schonbein, G. W. (1998).
Plasma hydrogen peroxide production in hypertensives and
normotensive subjects at genetic risk of hypertension. Journal
de l’hypertension, 16, 291–303.
Lawes, C. M., Bennett, D. A., Lewington, S., & Rodgers, A. (2002).
Blood pressure and coronary heart disease: a review of the
evidence. Seminars in Vascular Medicine., 2, 355–368.
Lewington, S., Clarke, R., Qizilbash, N., Peto, R., & Collins, R.
(2002). Age-specific relevance of usual blood pressure to
vascular mortality: a meta-analysis of individual data for one
million adults in 61 prospective studies. Lancet, 360, 1903–1913.
Li, L. J., Geng, S. R., & Yu, C. M. (2005). Endothelial dysfunction in
normotensive Chinese with a family history of essential
hypertension. Clinical and Experimental Hypertension, 27, 1–8.
Lindsay, J., Hebert, R., & Rockwood, K. (1997). The Canadian Study
of Health and Aging: risk factors for vascular dementia. Stroke,
28, 526–530.
Mazoyer, B., Zago, L., Mellet, E., Bricogne, S., Etard, O., Houde, O.,
et al. (2001). Cortical networks for working memory and
executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man.
Brain Research Bulletin, 54, 287–298.
McKiernan, K. A., D’Angelo, B. R., Kaufman, J. N., & Binder, J. R.
(2006). Interrupting the “stream of consciousness”: an fMRI
investigation. Neuroimage, 29, 1185–1191.
McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., & Binder,
J. R. (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-
Brain Imaging and Behavior (2008) 2:192–199
induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 394–408.
Moser, D. J., Cohen, R. A., Clark, M. M., Aloia, M. S., Tate, B. A.,
Stefanik, S., et al. (1999). Neuropsychological functioning
among cardiac rehabilitation patients. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation, 19, 91–97.
O’Brien, J. T., Erkinjuntti, T., Reisberg, B., Roman, G., Sawada, T.,
Pantoni, L., et al. (2003). Vascular cognitive impairment. Lancet
Neurology, 2, 89–98.
O’Connor, D. T., Kailasam, M. T., Kennedy, B. P., Ziegler, M. G.,
Yanaihara, N., & Parmer, R. J. (2002). Early decline in the catecholamine release-inhibitory peptide catestatin in humans at genetic
risk of hypertension. Journal de l’hypertension, 20, 1335–1345.
Page, G., & France, C. R. (2001). Identifying hypertension using the Ohio
Blood Pressure History Survey. Military Medicine, 166, 233–236.
Pantoni, L., & Garcia, J. H. (1997). Pathogenesis of leukoaraiosis: a
review. Stroke, 28, 652–659.
Paul, R. H., Gunstad, J., Poppas, A., Tate, D. F., Foreman, D.,
Brickman, A. M., et al. (2005). Neuroimaging and cardiac
correlates of cognitive function among patients with cardiac
disease. Cerebrovascular Diseases, 20, 129–133.
Penner, I. K., Rausch, M., Kappos, L., Opwis, K., & Radu, E. W.
(2003). Analysis of impairment related functional architecture in
MS patients during performance of different attention tasks.
Journal de neurologie, 250, 461–472.
Prince, M., Cullen, M., & Mann, A. (1994). Risk factors for
Alzheimer’s disease and dementia: a case-control study based
on the MRC elderly hypertension trial. Neurology, 44, 97–104.
Sacco, R. L., Wolf, P. A., & Gorelick, P. B. (1999). Risk factors and
their management for stroke prevention: outlook for 1999 and
beyond. Neurology, 53, S15–S24.
Schneider, G. M., Jacobs, D. W., Gevirtz, R. N., & O’Connor, D. T.
(2003). Cardiovascular haemodynamic response to repeated mental
stress in normotensive subjects at genetic risk of hypertension:
evidence of enhanced reactivity, blunted adaptation, and delayed
recovery. Journal of Human Hypertension, 17, 829–840.
Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M.,
Raichle, M. E., et al. (1997). Common blood flow changes across
visual tasks.2. Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 9, 648–663.
Smith, A. M., Fried, P. A., Hogan, M. J., & Cameron, I. (2006).
Effects of prenatal marijuana on visuospatial working memory:
an fMRI study in young adults. Neurotoxicology and Teratology,
28, 286–295.
199
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: a view from
neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33, 5–42.
Song, C. K., Martinez, J. A., Kailasam, M. T., Dao, T. T., Wong, C. M.,
Parmer, R. J., et al. (2000). Renal kallikrein excretion: role of
ethnicity, gender, environment, and genetic risk of hypertension.
Journal of Human Hypertension, 14, 461–468.
Staffen, W., Mair, A., Zauner, H., Unterrainer, J., Niederhofer, H.,
Kutzelnigg, A., et al. (2002). Cognitive function and fMRI in
patients with multiple sclerosis: evidence for compensatory
cortical activation during an attention task. Brain, 125, 1275–
1282.
Sweet, L. H., Paskavitz, J. F., Haley, A. P., Gunstad, J., Mulligan, R.,
Nyalakanti, P. K., et al. (2008). Imaging phonological similarity
effects on verbal working memory. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1114–
1123.
Sweet, L. H., Rao, S. M., Primeau, M., Durgerian, S., & Cohen, R. A.
(2006). Functional magnetic resonance imaging response to
increased verbal working memory demands among patients with
multiple sclerosis. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 28–36.
Sweet, L. H., Rao, S. M., Primeau, M., Mayer, A. R., & Cohen, R. A.
(2004). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of working
memory among multiple sclerosis patients. Journal of Neuroimaging, 14, 150–157.
Talairach, J., & Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of
the human brain 3-D proportional system: an approach to
cerebral imaging. New York: Thieme Medical.
Thyrum, E. T., Blumenthal, J. A., Madden, D. J., & Siegel, W. (1995).
Family history of hypertension influences neurobehavioral
function in hypertensive patients. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57,
496–500.
van Dijk, E. J., Breteler, M. M., Schmidt, R., Berger, K., Nilsson, L. G.,
Oudkerk, M., et al. (2004). The association between blood
pressure, hypertension, and cerebral white matter lesions:
cardiovascular determinants of dementia study. Hypertension,
44, 625–630.
Waldstein, S. R., Ryan, C. M., Polefrone, J. M., & Manuck, S. B.
(1994). Neuropsychological performance of young men who
vary in familial risk for hypertension. Psychosomatic Medicine,
56, 449–456.
Zhang, L., Rao, F., Wessel, J., Kennedy, B. P., Rana, B. K., Taupenot,
L., et al. (2004). Functional allelic heterogeneity and pleiotropy
of a repeat polymorphism in tyrosine hydroxylase: prediction of
catecholamines and response to stress in twins. Physiological
Genomics, 19, 277–291.