Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Comparative evidence for a cost to males of manipulating females in bushcrickets

Behavioral Ecology, 2007
...Read more
Comparative evidence for a cost to males of manipulating females in bushcrickets Karim Vahed Biological Sciences Research Group, Faculty of Education, Health and Sciences, University of Derby, Kedleston Road, Derby DE22 1GB, UK Recent theoretical and empirical research on sexual conflict has tended to focus on the costs to females of being manipulated by males. The costs to males associated with the production of manipulative traits have received relatively little attention. In numerous insects, including bushcrickets (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae), males are known to transfer substances in the ejaculate that inhibit the receptivity of females to further matings in a dose-dependent manner. The aim of this study was to test the prediction that, across bushcricket taxa, larger ejaculates and nuptial gifts will be associated with, on the one hand, longer sexual refractory periods in females and, on the other hand, longer sexual refractory periods in males. Data on the duration of the sexual refractory period in both males and females, together with ejaculate mass, spermatophylax mass, and male body mass, were obtained for 23 species of bushcricket. Both comparative analysis by independent contrasts and species regression revealed a positive relationship, across taxa, between the duration of the female’s sexual refractory period and both relative ejaculate mass and relative nuptial gift mass. Positive relationships were also found between the duration of the male’s sexual refractory period and both relative ejaculate mass and relative nuptial gift mass, indicating that there is a trade-off between resources spent on spermatophore size and the male’s potential mating rate. This appears to be the first comparative evidence that there is a cost to males associated with manipulating the remating behavior of their mates. Key words: mating costs, nuptial feeding, sexual conflict, sexual refractory period, sexually antagonistic coevolution. [Behav Ecol 18:499–506 (2007)] INTRODUCTION I t is becoming increasingly clear that mating and fertilization often involve a conflict of interest between the sexes be- cause male and female fitness optima may differ over, for example, female mating rate (reviewed by Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Hosken and Snook 2005; Tregenza et al. 2006). The recent proliferation of published research on sexual conflict in relation to mating has tended to focus on the costs to females of being manipulated by males, yet it is recognized that coevolutionary arms races between the sexes could lead to substantial fitness costs for both sexes (Gavrilets et al. 2001; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Ha ¨rdling and Smith 2005; Lessels 2006). The costs to males associated with the production of traits that may have been elaborated through a process of sexually antagonistic coevolution, however, have received rel- atively little attention recently (but see Macias-Garcia and Ramirez 2005; Stuart-Fox 2005). It has been proposed that large ejaculates and nuptial gifts in insects are the products of sexually antagonistic coevolu- tion (Arnqvist and Nilsson 2000; Sakaluk 2000; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Arnqvist 2006; Sakaluk et al. 2006; Vahed 2006, 2007). In many insects, substances in the ejaculate appear to manipulate the mating behavior of the female (reviewed by Cordero 1995; Arnqvist and Nilsson 2000; Simmons 2001; Wolfner 2002; Gillott 2003; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Wedell 2005), increasing the length of the females’ postmating sexual refractory period (the period of time after mating during which the female remains unreceptive to further mating) in a dose-dependent manner (e.g., Thornhill 1976, 1983; Gwynne 1986; Wedell and Arak 1989; Simmons and Gwynne 1991; Wedell 1993b, 2005; Torres-Vila et al. 1997; Bailey and Nuhadiyati 2005). Recent comparative evidence suggests that substances in large ejaculates can decrease the females’ lifetime degree of polyandry (Vahed 2006). Vahed (2006) found a negative relationship, across bushcricket taxa, be- tween ejaculate mass (relative to male body mass) and the lifetime degree of polyandry in bushcrickets. Such adaptations are likely to benefit the male by increas- ing his fertilization success in the face of sperm competition (Simmons 2001). A short-term reduction in receptivity after mating could also benefit the female, allowing her to avoid the risks of mate searching (e.g., see Heller 1992) and to concentrate on oviposition (Arnqvist and Rowe 2005). How- ever, a long-term reduction in receptivity may be costly for the female (Arnqvist and Nilsson 2000; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005). Females may suffer costs through being prevented from gain- ing possible direct benefits (e.g., nutrients or energy from nuptial gifts, reviewed by Vahed 1998b, 2007; Gwynne 2001) associated with polyandrous matings, together with the indi- rect (genetic) benefits (for reviews of the genetic benefits of polyandry, see Hosken and Stockley 2003; Simmons 2005). Furthermore, there may be direct survival costs associated with receiving manipulative male accessory gland products (Chapman et al. 1995; Rice et al. 2006). Therefore, females may have been selected to raise their response thresholds to ‘‘manipulative’’ ejaculate substances (see Simmons and Gwynne 1991), to digest excess ejaculate or to interrupt ejac- ulate transfer, resulting in a coevolutionary arms race selecting for increased ejaculate size (Arnqvist and Nilsson 2000; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Vahed 2006). In some insects, the male manufactures a nuptial gift that is eaten by the female during ejaculate transfer (reviewed by Vahed 1998b, 2007). In bushcrickets, for example, the nuptial gift consists of a gelati- nous mass (the spermatophylax) that is attached to the part of the spermatophore that contains the ejaculate (the ampulla) (reviewed in Vahed 1998b, 2007; Gwynne 2001). Such nuptial gifts may be seen as a means by which males can overcome the resistance of the female to accepting a large ejaculate because they deter the female from interfering with insemination Address correspondence to K. Vahed. E-mail: k.vahed@derby.ac.uk. Received 15 September 2006; revised 1 December 2006; accepted 17 December 2006. Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arm021 Advance Access publication 30 March 2007 Ó The Author 2007. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org by guest on September 30, 2015 http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ Downloaded from
(Boldyrev 1915; Vahed and Gilbert 1996; Vahed 1998b, 2006, 2007). There are, however, costs incurred by males producing large ejaculates and nuptial gifts. Both models of ejaculate expenditure (e.g., Parker and Ball 2005) and models of nup- tial gift allocation (e.g., Kondoh 2001) assume that there will be a trade-off in males between resources spent on current reproduction and lifetime mating rate. This trade-off has profound implications in terms of the evolution of male mate choice (reviewed by Clutton-Brock and Parker 1992; Bonduriansky 2001; Gwynne 2001; Simmons and Kvarnemo 2006) and strategic ejaculate allocation (reviewed by Wedell et al. 2002; see also Parker and Ball 2005). However, few em- pirical studies have tested the assumption that such a trade-off does occur. Energetic costs to males of producing large ejac- ulates and nuptial gifts are often inferred from the observa- tion that males of insect species with relatively large ejaculates and nuptial gifts have long sexual refractory periods (e.g., Simmons 1990, 1993; Heller and von Helversen 1991; Hayashi 1993; Sakaluk et al. 2004), during which time the male appears to begin to replenish glands which secrete the sper- matophore and the nuptial gift (Davies and Dadour 1989; Simmons 1990, 1993, 1995; Heller and von Helversen 1991; Simmons et al. 1992, 1999; Hayashi 1993; Reinhold and von Helversen 1997; Lehmann GUC and Lehmann AW 2000b). As yet, however, there has been no formal comparative analysis of the effects of relative ejaculate size and nuptial gift size on the duration of the sexual refractory period in males, other than Wedell (1994), who found that bushcricket species with male refractory periods greater than 2 days in length tend also to have a relatively high protein content in the spermatophylax. The aims of the present study were 1) to examine a cost to males of producing large ejaculates in bushcrickets, that is, to test the prediction that bushcricket species with relatively larger nuptial gifts and ejaculates will have longer sexual re- fractory periods in males and 2) to examine, for the same set of bushcricket species, a benefit of producing large ejaculates and nuptial gifts, that is, to test the prediction that relatively larger ejaculates and nuptial gifts are associated with the in- duction of longer sexual refractory periods in females across species. The latter prediction has previously been tested in tettigoniids by Wedell (1993b), who found positive relation- ships across genera between the duration of the females’ sexual refractory periods and the absolute mass of both the nuptial gift and ejaculate. It has been proposed, however, that the results of Wedell (1993b) may have been confounded by phylogeny because this factor was not controlled for (Gwynne 2001). MATERIALS AND METHODS A range of bushcricket species (Table 1) were collected from the field, as adults, from a variety of European locations (primarily France, Spain, and UK; for details, see Vahed and Gilbert 1996; Vahed 2006). Sexes were separated and main- tained under laboratory conditions as outlined by Hartley and Dean (1974). Data on the duration of the sexual refractory periods for males and females were obtained both from wild- caught individuals and from offspring subsequently reared in the laboratory (for further details of the treatment of the egg stage, see Hartley 1990). For each species, individual adults were uniquely marked using enamel paint. Matings were set up in black nylon mesh observation cages (measuring 10 3 10 3 10 cm), one pair per cage. In order to determine the duration of the sexual refractory period for the males, immediately after mating, the male was left in the observation cage and was presented with a fresh, sexually receptive female (i.e., one that had been showing signs of phonotaxis to the male’s call or exhibiting a response song where present). The pairs were then observed for at least 5 h. If the male did not show renewed sexual activity, he was returned to the rearing cage before being presented with a fresh female on each subsequent day (for a period of at least 5 h) until sexual activity by the male was resumed. These pairings were set up at a time of day corresponding to the peak of sexual activity for the given species. This varied be- tween species. For example, some species were more active after sunset (e.g., Uromenus rugosicollis and Ruspolia nitidula), whereas others were more active during midmorning to mid- afternoon (e.g., Anonconotus alpinus and Steropleurus spp). Al- though the resumption of tegminal stridulation (calling) by the male could be used as an indicator that the male is ready to mate, observations suggested that this may not always be a reliable sign. In the tettigoniine bushcrickets observed, males would often give brief bursts of stridulation within 5 min of mating, even though, in most cases, the males were not ready to mate again. Similarly, in Leptophyes laticauda, males would engage in ‘‘aggressive stridulation’’ (see Hartley 1991) before they were ready to remate. Therefore, males were only recorded as having regained sexual activity when they adopted the precopulatory position in the presence of a female. In this behavior, the male turns his abdomen to face the female’s head, bending his abdomen downward to expose the secretory glands situated on his dorsal tergites which are used to entice the female to copulate (see Engelhardt 1915). To determine the refractory periods of the females, a similar method was employed. The newly mated female was pre- sented with a fresh, sexually receptive male for at least 5 h after mating at a time of day corresponding to the peak of mating activity for the species. If mating did not occur in this period, the female was returned to the rearing cage before being presented with a different sexually receptive male (and observed for at least 5 h) on each subsequent day until cop- ulation occurred. Novel data on the sexual refractory periods of males were collected for 12 species of tettigoniid, and data for a further 11 were obtained from the literature (Table 1). Novel data on the sexual refractory periods of females were obtained for 9 species, whereas data for a further 14 were taken from the literature. Data on mean ampulla mass, nuptial gift mass, and male mass for all 23 species were taken from the literature (Table 1; NB: there appear to be 22 species in this table be- cause data for Metrioptera roeselii and Metrioptera bicolor were combined. This was done because the data on the duration of the female refractory period taken from Wedell [1993b] were based on the mean of the mean values for both species). An assumption of this study is that differences in ampulla mass between species reflect differences in ejaculate mass or volume. Although this is likely to be the case, it is conceivable that some of the variation in ampulla mass across species is due to differences in the thickness of the ampulla wall, rather than ejaculate volume. Data were analyzed using both inde- pendent contrasts to control for phylogeny (for details, see Harvey and Pagel 1991; Purvis and Rambaut 1995) and species regression. Data were log 10 transformed to meet the assump- tions of parametric linear regression. The phylogeny used in the analysis by independent contrasts was taken primarily from Naskrecki (2000). The phylogenetic relationships of species within the genus Poecilimon were taken from Heller (1984, 2006), whereas the relationships between the different genera in the subfamily Tettigoniinae were taken from Rentz and Colless (1990) (this is because some of the genera from this subfamily in the present study were not included in Naskrecki’s [2000] phylogeny). Because information on branch lengths was not available, the contrasts were not scaled. To control for allometry, analyses were performed on 500 Behavioral Ecology by guest on September 30, 2015 http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ Downloaded from
Behavioral Ecology doi:10.1093/beheco/arm021 Advance Access publication 30 March 2007 Comparative evidence for a cost to males of manipulating females in bushcrickets Karim Vahed Biological Sciences Research Group, Faculty of Education, Health and Sciences, University of Derby, Kedleston Road, Derby DE22 1GB, UK INTRODUCTION I t is becoming increasingly clear that mating and fertilization often involve a conflict of interest between the sexes because male and female fitness optima may differ over, for example, female mating rate (reviewed by Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Hosken and Snook 2005; Tregenza et al. 2006). The recent proliferation of published research on sexual conflict in relation to mating has tended to focus on the costs to females of being manipulated by males, yet it is recognized that coevolutionary arms races between the sexes could lead to substantial fitness costs for both sexes (Gavrilets et al. 2001; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Härdling and Smith 2005; Lessels 2006). The costs to males associated with the production of traits that may have been elaborated through a process of sexually antagonistic coevolution, however, have received relatively little attention recently (but see Macias-Garcia and Ramirez 2005; Stuart-Fox 2005). It has been proposed that large ejaculates and nuptial gifts in insects are the products of sexually antagonistic coevolution (Arnqvist and Nilsson 2000; Sakaluk 2000; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Arnqvist 2006; Sakaluk et al. 2006; Vahed 2006, 2007). In many insects, substances in the ejaculate appear to manipulate the mating behavior of the female (reviewed by Cordero 1995; Arnqvist and Nilsson 2000; Simmons 2001; Wolfner 2002; Gillott 2003; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Wedell 2005), increasing the length of the females’ postmating sexual refractory period (the period of time after mating during which the female remains unreceptive to further mating) in a dose-dependent manner (e.g., Thornhill 1976, 1983; Gwynne 1986; Wedell and Arak 1989; Simmons and Gwynne 1991; Wedell 1993b, 2005; Torres-Vila et al. 1997; Bailey and Nuhadiyati 2005). Recent comparative evidence suggests that Address correspondence to K. Vahed. E-mail: k.vahed@derby.ac.uk. Received 15 September 2006; revised 1 December 2006; accepted 17 December 2006.  The Author 2007. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oxfordjournals.org substances in large ejaculates can decrease the females’ lifetime degree of polyandry (Vahed 2006). Vahed (2006) found a negative relationship, across bushcricket taxa, between ejaculate mass (relative to male body mass) and the lifetime degree of polyandry in bushcrickets. Such adaptations are likely to benefit the male by increasing his fertilization success in the face of sperm competition (Simmons 2001). A short-term reduction in receptivity after mating could also benefit the female, allowing her to avoid the risks of mate searching (e.g., see Heller 1992) and to concentrate on oviposition (Arnqvist and Rowe 2005). However, a long-term reduction in receptivity may be costly for the female (Arnqvist and Nilsson 2000; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005). Females may suffer costs through being prevented from gaining possible direct benefits (e.g., nutrients or energy from nuptial gifts, reviewed by Vahed 1998b, 2007; Gwynne 2001) associated with polyandrous matings, together with the indirect (genetic) benefits (for reviews of the genetic benefits of polyandry, see Hosken and Stockley 2003; Simmons 2005). Furthermore, there may be direct survival costs associated with receiving manipulative male accessory gland products (Chapman et al. 1995; Rice et al. 2006). Therefore, females may have been selected to raise their response thresholds to ‘‘manipulative’’ ejaculate substances (see Simmons and Gwynne 1991), to digest excess ejaculate or to interrupt ejaculate transfer, resulting in a coevolutionary arms race selecting for increased ejaculate size (Arnqvist and Nilsson 2000; Arnqvist and Rowe 2005; Vahed 2006). In some insects, the male manufactures a nuptial gift that is eaten by the female during ejaculate transfer (reviewed by Vahed 1998b, 2007). In bushcrickets, for example, the nuptial gift consists of a gelatinous mass (the spermatophylax) that is attached to the part of the spermatophore that contains the ejaculate (the ampulla) (reviewed in Vahed 1998b, 2007; Gwynne 2001). Such nuptial gifts may be seen as a means by which males can overcome the resistance of the female to accepting a large ejaculate because they deter the female from interfering with insemination Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 30, 2015 Recent theoretical and empirical research on sexual conflict has tended to focus on the costs to females of being manipulated by males. The costs to males associated with the production of manipulative traits have received relatively little attention. In numerous insects, including bushcrickets (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae), males are known to transfer substances in the ejaculate that inhibit the receptivity of females to further matings in a dose-dependent manner. The aim of this study was to test the prediction that, across bushcricket taxa, larger ejaculates and nuptial gifts will be associated with, on the one hand, longer sexual refractory periods in females and, on the other hand, longer sexual refractory periods in males. Data on the duration of the sexual refractory period in both males and females, together with ejaculate mass, spermatophylax mass, and male body mass, were obtained for 23 species of bushcricket. Both comparative analysis by independent contrasts and species regression revealed a positive relationship, across taxa, between the duration of the female’s sexual refractory period and both relative ejaculate mass and relative nuptial gift mass. Positive relationships were also found between the duration of the male’s sexual refractory period and both relative ejaculate mass and relative nuptial gift mass, indicating that there is a trade-off between resources spent on spermatophore size and the male’s potential mating rate. This appears to be the first comparative evidence that there is a cost to males associated with manipulating the remating behavior of their mates. Key words: mating costs, nuptial feeding, sexual conflict, sexual refractory period, sexually antagonistic coevolution. [Behav Ecol 18:499–506 (2007)] 500 MATERIALS AND METHODS A range of bushcricket species (Table 1) were collected from the field, as adults, from a variety of European locations (primarily France, Spain, and UK; for details, see Vahed and Gilbert 1996; Vahed 2006). Sexes were separated and maintained under laboratory conditions as outlined by Hartley and Dean (1974). Data on the duration of the sexual refractory periods for males and females were obtained both from wildcaught individuals and from offspring subsequently reared in the laboratory (for further details of the treatment of the egg stage, see Hartley 1990). For each species, individual adults were uniquely marked using enamel paint. Matings were set up in black nylon mesh observation cages (measuring 10 3 10 3 10 cm), one pair per cage. In order to determine the duration of the sexual refractory period for the males, immediately after mating, the male was left in the observation cage and was presented with a fresh, sexually receptive female (i.e., one that had been showing signs of phonotaxis to the male’s call or exhibiting a response song where present). The pairs were then observed for at least 5 h. If the male did not show renewed sexual activity, he was returned to the rearing cage before being presented with a fresh female on each subsequent day (for a period of at least 5 h) until sexual activity by the male was resumed. These pairings were set up at a time of day corresponding to the peak of sexual activity for the given species. This varied between species. For example, some species were more active after sunset (e.g., Uromenus rugosicollis and Ruspolia nitidula), whereas others were more active during midmorning to midafternoon (e.g., Anonconotus alpinus and Steropleurus spp). Although the resumption of tegminal stridulation (calling) by the male could be used as an indicator that the male is ready to mate, observations suggested that this may not always be a reliable sign. In the tettigoniine bushcrickets observed, males would often give brief bursts of stridulation within 5 min of mating, even though, in most cases, the males were not ready to mate again. Similarly, in Leptophyes laticauda, males would engage in ‘‘aggressive stridulation’’ (see Hartley 1991) before they were ready to remate. Therefore, males were only recorded as having regained sexual activity when they adopted the precopulatory position in the presence of a female. In this behavior, the male turns his abdomen to face the female’s head, bending his abdomen downward to expose the secretory glands situated on his dorsal tergites which are used to entice the female to copulate (see Engelhardt 1915). To determine the refractory periods of the females, a similar method was employed. The newly mated female was presented with a fresh, sexually receptive male for at least 5 h after mating at a time of day corresponding to the peak of mating activity for the species. If mating did not occur in this period, the female was returned to the rearing cage before being presented with a different sexually receptive male (and observed for at least 5 h) on each subsequent day until copulation occurred. Novel data on the sexual refractory periods of males were collected for 12 species of tettigoniid, and data for a further 11 were obtained from the literature (Table 1). Novel data on the sexual refractory periods of females were obtained for 9 species, whereas data for a further 14 were taken from the literature. Data on mean ampulla mass, nuptial gift mass, and male mass for all 23 species were taken from the literature (Table 1; NB: there appear to be 22 species in this table because data for Metrioptera roeselii and Metrioptera bicolor were combined. This was done because the data on the duration of the female refractory period taken from Wedell [1993b] were based on the mean of the mean values for both species). An assumption of this study is that differences in ampulla mass between species reflect differences in ejaculate mass or volume. Although this is likely to be the case, it is conceivable that some of the variation in ampulla mass across species is due to differences in the thickness of the ampulla wall, rather than ejaculate volume. Data were analyzed using both independent contrasts to control for phylogeny (for details, see Harvey and Pagel 1991; Purvis and Rambaut 1995) and species regression. Data were log10 transformed to meet the assumptions of parametric linear regression. The phylogeny used in the analysis by independent contrasts was taken primarily from Naskrecki (2000). The phylogenetic relationships of species within the genus Poecilimon were taken from Heller (1984, 2006), whereas the relationships between the different genera in the subfamily Tettigoniinae were taken from Rentz and Colless (1990) (this is because some of the genera from this subfamily in the present study were not included in Naskrecki’s [2000] phylogeny). Because information on branch lengths was not available, the contrasts were not scaled. To control for allometry, analyses were performed on Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 30, 2015 (Boldyrev 1915; Vahed and Gilbert 1996; Vahed 1998b, 2006, 2007). There are, however, costs incurred by males producing large ejaculates and nuptial gifts. Both models of ejaculate expenditure (e.g., Parker and Ball 2005) and models of nuptial gift allocation (e.g., Kondoh 2001) assume that there will be a trade-off in males between resources spent on current reproduction and lifetime mating rate. This trade-off has profound implications in terms of the evolution of male mate choice (reviewed by Clutton-Brock and Parker 1992; Bonduriansky 2001; Gwynne 2001; Simmons and Kvarnemo 2006) and strategic ejaculate allocation (reviewed by Wedell et al. 2002; see also Parker and Ball 2005). However, few empirical studies have tested the assumption that such a trade-off does occur. Energetic costs to males of producing large ejaculates and nuptial gifts are often inferred from the observation that males of insect species with relatively large ejaculates and nuptial gifts have long sexual refractory periods (e.g., Simmons 1990, 1993; Heller and von Helversen 1991; Hayashi 1993; Sakaluk et al. 2004), during which time the male appears to begin to replenish glands which secrete the spermatophore and the nuptial gift (Davies and Dadour 1989; Simmons 1990, 1993, 1995; Heller and von Helversen 1991; Simmons et al. 1992, 1999; Hayashi 1993; Reinhold and von Helversen 1997; Lehmann GUC and Lehmann AW 2000b). As yet, however, there has been no formal comparative analysis of the effects of relative ejaculate size and nuptial gift size on the duration of the sexual refractory period in males, other than Wedell (1994), who found that bushcricket species with male refractory periods greater than 2 days in length tend also to have a relatively high protein content in the spermatophylax. The aims of the present study were 1) to examine a cost to males of producing large ejaculates in bushcrickets, that is, to test the prediction that bushcricket species with relatively larger nuptial gifts and ejaculates will have longer sexual refractory periods in males and 2) to examine, for the same set of bushcricket species, a benefit of producing large ejaculates and nuptial gifts, that is, to test the prediction that relatively larger ejaculates and nuptial gifts are associated with the induction of longer sexual refractory periods in females across species. The latter prediction has previously been tested in tettigoniids by Wedell (1993b), who found positive relationships across genera between the duration of the females’ sexual refractory periods and the absolute mass of both the nuptial gift and ejaculate. It has been proposed, however, that the results of Wedell (1993b) may have been confounded by phylogeny because this factor was not controlled for (Gwynne 2001). Behavioral Ecology Vahed • A cost of manipulating females 501 Table 1 Mean duration of the sexual refractory period in males and in females (under normal, as opposed to food limited, conditions), together with male body mass, ampulla mass, and spermatophylax (sp’lax) mass for the different tettigoniid species (sample sizes are in parentheses) Taxon Phaneropterinae 1. Phaneroptera nana 2. Leptophyes punctatissima 3. Leptophyes laticauda 4. Poecilimon schmidtii 5. Poecilimon veluchianus 6. Poecilimon affinis 7. Poecilimon mariannae Conocephalinae 13. Conocephalus discolor 14. Ruspolia nitidula 104.0 (3)a 24.0 (20)k 1.0 (3)a 0.2 (12)l 1.38 (2 1 2)a Male mass (mg) Ampulla mass (mg) Sp’lax mass (mg) 24.0 (5)a 3.09 (7)a 165.6 (15)a 51.6 (11)a 72.0 (249)c 48.0 (860)c,g 165.6 (10)i 289.0 175.0 478.0 525.0 630.0 1328.0 584.4 (7)b (15)b (17)b (8)b (34)e (4)b (21)h 5.24 (7)b 1.05 (10)b 20.28 (17)b 9.17 (6)b 31.3 (13)f 30.89 (3)b 24.0 (21)h 9.16 (7)b 5.97 (14)b 103.65 (17)b 63.39 (6)b 118.6 (13)f 170.27 (4)b 109.04 (21)h 172.8 (27)j 110.4 (263)j 24.0 (3)a 1.07 (5)l 76.8 (27 1 31)j 1805.0 1618.0 479.0 604.0 391.5 (27)j (3)b (3)b (6)b (3 1 3)b 61.8 (27)j 56.09 (3)b 12.2 (3)b 4.94 (6)b 17.48 (3 1 3)b 188.5 (27)j 123.42 (3)b 14.37 (3)b 7.71 (6)b 22.0 (3 1 3)b 1.03 (9)a 0.04 (7)a 96.0 (5)a 93.6 (10)a 150.0 (2)b 556.0 (3)b Hetrodinae 15. Acanthoplus discoidalis 72.0 (40)m 171.84 (22)m 4650.0 (14)m Bradyporinae 16. Ephippiger ephippiger 17. Steropleurus stali 18. Steropleurus asturiensis 19. Uromenus rugosicollis 105.6 (5)a 72.03 (13)n 120.0 (6)a 24.0 (10)n Listroscelidinae 20. Requena verticalis Zaprochilinae 21. Kawanaphila nartee Phasmodinae 22. Phasmodes ranatriformis a 3.15 (3)a 1.17 (7)a 54.0 (12)a 26.42 (7)a 72.0 (64)c,d 48.0 (25)c,d 48.0 (21)h Female refractory period (h) 115.2 115.2 96.0 72.0 (23)j (20) o (6)a (3)a 62.4 (8)q 96.0 (24)r 128.88 (16)t 456.0 (11)u 24.0 (4)w 43.2 (5)j 2313.0 1290.0 1210.0 1143.0 (5)b (51)n (10)n (9)b 440.0 (8)q 55.0 (10)t 234.8 (5)j 2.38 (2)b 2.21 (3)b 12.2 (2)b 1.59 (3)b 280.0 (14)m 650.0 (14)m 148.97 (6)b 55.35 (17)p 60.5 (10)n 62.72 (9)b 468.76 (5)b 322.0 (17)p 270.0 (10)n 79.01 (9)b 13.07 (39)s 36.2 (39)s 5.95 (1)v 5.76 (1)v 9.3 (5)j Novel data. Data from Vahed and Gilbert (1996). c Data from Heller and von Helversen 1991. d Time until .90% of males remated. e Data from Reinhold and Helversen (1997). f Data from Heller et al. (1998). g Median remating interval (mean not given). h Data from Lehmann GUC and Lehmann AW (2000a). i Data from Lehmann GUC and Lehmann AW (2000b). j Data from Wedell (1993b). k Data from Wedell (1993a). l Data from Vahed (2002). m Data from Bateman and Ferguson (2004). n Data from Vahed (1997). o Data from Vahed (1998a). p Data from Bateman (1998). q Data from Gwynne (1990). r Data from Gwynne (1986). s Data from Simmons et al. (1993). t Data from Simmons (1990). u Data from Simmons and Gwynne (1991). v Simmons LW (personal communication). w Bailey and Lebel (1998). x Estimated value (spermatophylax is vestigial, comparable in relative mass to that of Ruspolia according to Gwynne 2001). b ;1.0x Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 30, 2015 Tettigoniinae 8. Tettigonia viridissima 9. Decticus verrucivorus 10. Platycleis albopunctata 11. Anonconotus alpinus 12. Metrioptera spp (bicolor 1 roeselii) Male refractory period (h) Behavioral Ecology 502 2.5 a 6 1.5 22 Contrasts in female refractory period Log male refractory period (h) 2.0 4 19 9 1.0 1 .5 12 2 10 13 0.0 -.5 2.0 21 18 16 8 5 15 3 20 17 7 11 -1.0 1.5 1.0 .5 0.0 -.5 -1.0 14 -1.5 -1.5 -.5 0.0 .5 1.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 -.8 3.0 -.6 -.4 RESULTS The mean duration of the sexual refractory period in males for each species varied from only 2.4 min in R. nitidula and 12 min in A. alpinus to approximately 5 days in Steropleurus asturiensis and Kawanaphila nartee (Table 1, Figure 1). The mean interval between successive copulations for females of each species varied from 1.07 h in A. alpinus and 3.09 h in L. punctatissima to approximately 7 days in Tettigonia viridissima and Acanthoplus discoidalis and 19 days in K. nartee (Table 1, Figure 1). Comparative analysis by independent contrasts Contrasts in log spermatophylax mass were positively related to contrasts in log male body mass (slope ¼ 1.12 6 0.30, F1,21 ¼ 13.77, P , 0.01), as were contrasts in log ampulla mass (slope ¼ 1.05 6 0.18, F1,21 ¼ 33.76, P , 0.001). A positive relationship was found between residual contrasts in spermatophylax mass and residual contrasts ampulla mass (slope ¼ 1.21 6 0.25, F1,21 ¼ 23.04, P , 0.001). There was no significant relationship between contrasts in log duration of the male refractory period and contrasts in male body mass (F1,21 ¼ 1.02, P ¼ 0.33) or between contrasts in log duration of the female refractory period and contrasts in male body mass (F1,21 ¼ 0.23, P ¼ 0.64). As predicted, positive relationships were found between contrasts in the duration of the female refractory period and both contrasts in residual ampulla mass (slope ¼ 1.40 6 0.30, b Contrasts in female refractory period residuals from the least-squares regressions of contrasts in log ampulla mass and contrasts in log spermatophylax mass on contrasts in log male body mass. Stepwise regression (backward deletion method) was used to determine the effects of residual contrasts in log ampulla mass and residual contrasts in log spermatophylax mass on 1) contrasts in the duration of the male refractory period and 2) contrasts in the duration of the female refractory period. The analysis using species means, as opposed to contrasts, as independent data points was performed in a similar manner to the above. For the independent contrasts, all regressions were forced through the origin, as recommended by Harvey and Pagel (1991) and Purvis and Rambaut (1995). 0.0 .2 .4 .6 .8 1.0 2.0 1.5 1.0 .5 0.0 -.5 -1.0 -1.5 -2.0 -1.5 -1.0 -.5 0.0 .5 1.0 Residual contrasts in spermatophylax mass Figure 2 A benefit to males of producing large spermatophores: positive relationships between contrasts in the duration of the sexual refractory period in females and both (a) contrasts in residual ampulla (ejaculate) mass and (b) contrasts in residual nuptial gift mass across bushcricket taxa. F1,21 ¼ 21.92, P , 0.001; Figure 2a) and contrasts in residual spermatophylax mass (slope ¼ 0.64 6 0.22, F1,21 ¼ 8.85, P , 0.01; Figure 2b). Stepwise regression (backward deletion method) revealed that residual ampulla mass contrasts were the best predictor of contrasts in the duration of the female refractory period. Removal of residual spermatophylax mass contrasts caused no significant change in the fit of the model (F change ¼ 0.05, P ¼ 0.82). Positive relationships were also found, as predicted, between contrasts in the duration of the male refractory period and both residual ampulla mass contrasts (slope ¼ 2.11 6 0.32, F1,21 ¼ 43.80, P , 0.001; Figure 3a) and residual spermatophylax mass contrasts (slope ¼ 1.12 6 0.23, F1,21 ¼ 26.95, P , 0.001; Figure 3b). Stepwise regression (backward deletion method) revealed that variation across taxa in both residual ampulla mass contrasts and residual spermatophylax mass contrasts explained a significant proportion of variation in male refractory period contrasts (contrasts in male refractory period ¼ 1.5 (60.44) residual ampulla mass contrasts 1 0.50 (60.27) residual spermatophylax mass contrasts, F2,21 ¼ 26.47, P , 0.001). Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 30, 2015 Figure 1 The relationship between the duration of the sexual refractory period in males and the duration of the sexual refractory period in females across bushcricket species. Numbers correspond to the species listed in Table 1. -.2 Residual contrasts in ampulla mass Log female refractory period (h) Vahed • A cost of manipulating females Contrasts in male refractory period a 503 2.0 1.0 0.0 -1.0 -2.0 -3.0 -.8 Contrasts in male refractory period -.4 -.2 0.0 .2 .4 .6 Residual contrasts in ampulla mass .8 1.0 2.0 1.0 DISCUSSION 0.0 -1.0 -2.0 -3.0 -2.0 -1.5 -1.0 -.5 0.0 .5 1.0 Residual contrasts in spermatophylax mass Figure 3 A cost to males of producing large spermatophores: positive relationships between contrasts in the duration of the sexual refractory period in males and both a) contrasts in residual ampulla (ejaculate) mass and b) contrasts in residual nuptial gift mass across bushcricket taxa. Species regression Across species, there was a positive relationship between log spermatophylax mass and log male body mass (log spermatophylax mass ¼ 2.30 1 1.41 log male body mass; r2 ¼ 0.60, F1,21 ¼ 29.94, P , 0.001) and between log ampulla mass and log male body mass (log ampulla mass ¼ 1.99 1 1.16 log male body mass; r2 ¼ 0.70, F1,21 ¼ 44.25, P , 0.001). There was no significant relationship between log duration of the male refractory period and male body mass (r2 ¼ 0.10, F1,21 ¼ 2.33, P ¼ 0.14) or between log duration of the female refractory period and male body mass (r2 ¼ 0.02, F1,21 ¼ 0.48, P ¼ 0.50). A positive relationship was found between residual spermatophylax mass and residual ampulla mass (residual spermatophylax mass ¼ 0.9 residual ampulla mass; r2 ¼ 0.37, F1,21 ¼ 11.76, P , 0.01). As predicted, positive relationships were found, across species, between the duration of the female refractory period and residual ampulla mass (log female refractory period ¼ 1.80 1 1.10 residual ampulla mass; r2 ¼ 0.42, F1,21 ¼ 14.54, P , 0.001) and between the duration of the female refractory period and Both species regression and comparative analysis by independent contrasts revealed, as predicted, a positive relationship between the duration of the male’s postmating sexual refractory period and both relative ejaculate size (ampulla mass) and nuptial gift (spermatophylax) mass. At one extreme, the male’s sexual refractory period lasted only a matter of minutes in R. nitidula and A. alpinus, species in which the spermatophore (ampulla 1 spermatophylax) represents only 2%, or less, of male body mass. At the other extreme, the male’s sexual refractory period lasted for 5 days on average in S. asturiensis and K. nartee, species in which the spermatophore represents more than 20% of male body mass. This study therefore supports the often-made assumption that there is a trade-off in males between resources spent on current reproduction and potential mating rate (e.g., Kondoh 2001; Parker and Ball 2005). The results also add to the growing body of evidence that males suffer costs as a result of the production of traits that may have been elaborated through a process of sexually antagonistic coevolution. Other examples of costly manipulative or coercive male traits include conspicuous prey-mimicking tail patterns in Goodeinine fish (Macias-Garcia and Ramirez 2005; Stuart-Fox 2005), sand pillars in fiddler crabs (Backwell et al. 1995), and mate-grasping devices in water striders (Arnqvist 1994; Westlake and Rowe 1999). Such costs associated with the production of manipulative traits or armaments are predicted to limit coevolutionary arms races between the sexes (Härdling and Smith 2005). To balance such costs, the production of relatively larger ejaculates was found to benefit the male by delaying the female from remating: as predicted, a positive relationship was found across bushcricket taxa between the duration of the female’s postmating sexual refractory period and the relative mass of both the ampulla and spermatophylax, as previously demonstrated by Wedell (1993b). The existence of such a relationship suggests that buschricket females appear not to have evolved complete resistance to refractory-inducing substances transferred by males (see also Simmons and Gwynne 1991; Sakaluk et al. 2006). Multiple regression analysis revealed that relative ampulla mass (as opposed to relative spermatophylax mass) was the best predictor of the duration of the female’s sexual refractory period. Vahed (2006) similarly found that relative ampulla mass (as opposed to either relative Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 30, 2015 b -.6 residual spermatophylax mass (log female refractory period ¼ 1.80 1 0.52 log spermatophylax mass; r2 ¼ 0.20, F1,21 ¼ 5.05, P , 0.05). Stepwise regression (backward deletion method) revealed that residual ampulla mass was the best predictor of the duration of the female refractory period. Removal of residual spermatophylax mass caused no significant change in the fit of the model (F change ¼ 0.15, P ¼ 0.70). Positive relationships were also found, as predicted, between the duration of the male refractory period and both residual ampulla mass (log male refractory period ¼ 1.13 1 2.29 residual ampulla mass; r2 ¼ 0.60, F1,21 ¼ 29.96, P , 0.001) and residual spermatophylax mass (log male refractory period ¼ 1.13 1 1.37 residual spermatophylax mass; r2 ¼ 0.47, F1,21 ¼ 17.48, P , 0.001). Stepwise regression (backward deletion method) revealed that interspecific variation in the duration of the male refractory period was explained by both residual ampulla mass and residual spermatophylax mass (log male refractory period ¼ 1.13 1 1.68 residual ampulla mass 1 0.68 residual spermatophylax mass; r2 ¼ 0.67, F2,21 ¼ 19.36, P , 0.001). There was a significant correlation, across species, between the duration of the male refractory period and the duration of the female refractory period (Pearson’s r ¼ 0.60, n ¼ 22, P , 0.01; Figure 1). Behavioral Ecology 504 2006). Therefore, although the elaboration of spermatophore size through selection on males to delay their mates from remating may have initially represented sexually antagonistic coevolution, in species with relatively large spermatophores, such a process could be seen as representing sexually mutualistic coevolution (see Macias-Garcia and Ramirez 2005; Stuart-Fox 2005). It remains to be determined, however, whether such indirect genetic benefits, together with direct nutritional benefits, associated with large spermatophores (reviewed in Vahed 1998b, 2007; Gwynne 2001) outweigh the costs to bushcricket females of being delayed, or prevented (see Vahed 2006), from engaging in polyandrous matings. I am grateful to Dr D.T. Gwynne, Dr K-G. Heller, and Dr A.D. Higginson for helpful discussions and for Dr D.T. Gwynne and Dr K-G. Heller for supplying information. I thank Dr D.T. Gwynne and Dr A.D. Higginson and 2 anonymous referees for their constructive comments on a previous version of the manuscript and Mr S. Hodson for assistance with graphics. REFERENCES Arnqvist G. 1994. The cost of male secondary sexual traits: developmental constraints during ontogeny in a sexually dimorphic water strider. Am Nat. 144:119–132. Arnqvist G. 2006. Sensory exploitation and sexual conflict. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 361:375–386. Arnqvist G, Nilsson T. 2000. The evolution of polyandry: multiple mating and female fitness in insects. Anim Behav. 60:145–164. Arnqvist G, Rowe L. 2005. Sexual conflict. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press. Backwell PRY, Jennions MD, Christy JH, Schober U. 1995. Pillar building in the fiddler crab Uca beebei—evidence for a condition dependent ornament. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 36:185–192. Bailey WJ, Lebel L. 1998. The mating biology of Phasmodes ranatriformis (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) a mute genus of bushcricket from Western Australia. J R Soc West Aust. 81:149–155. Bailey WJ, Nuhardiyati M. 2005. Copulation, the dynamics of sperm transfer and female refractoriness in the leafhopper Balclutha incisa (Hemiptera: Cicadellidae: Deltocephalinae). Physiol Entomol. 30: 343–352. Bateman PW. 1998. Does size matter? The function of the large spermatophore of Steropleurus stali Bolivar (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae: Ephippigerinae). J Orthop Res. 7:209–212. Bateman PW, Ferguson JWH. 2004. Male mate choice and spermatophore size in the Botswana armoured ground cricket Acanthoplus discoidalis. Can, and how, do males judge female mating history? J Zool Lond. 262:305–309. Boldyrev BT. 1915. Contributions à l’étude de la structure des spermatophores et des particularitès de la copulation chez Locustodea et Gryllodea. Horae Soc Ent Rossicae. 6:1–245. Bonduriansky R. 2001. The evolution of male mate choice in insects: a synthesis of ideas and evidence. Biol Rev. 76:306–339. Chapman T, Arnqvist G, Bangham J, Rowe L. 2003a. Response to Eberhard and Cordero, and Cordoba-Aguilar and ContrerasGarduno: sexual conflict and female choice. Trends Ecol Evol. 18:440–441. Chapman T, Arnqvist G, Bangham J, Rowe L. 2003b. Sexual conflict. Trends Ecol Evol. 18:41–47. Chapman T, Liddle LF, Kalb JM, Wolfner MF, Partridge L. 1995. Cost of mating in Drosophila melanogaster females is mediated via male accessory gland products. Nature. 373:241–244. Clutton-Brock TH, Parker GA. 1992. Potential reproductive rates and the operation of sexual selection. Q Rev Biol. 67:437–456. Cordero C. 1995. Ejaculate substances that affect female insect reproductive physiology and behaviour: honest or arbitrary traits? J Theor Biol. 174:453–461. Cordero C. 1996. On the evolutionary origin of nuptial seminal gifts in insects. J Insect Behav. 9:969–974. Cordero C, Eberhard WG. 2003. Female choice of sexually antagonistic male adaptations: a critical review of some current research. J Evol Biol. 16:1–6. Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 30, 2015 spermatophylax mass or relative sperm number) was the best predictor of the lifetime degree of polyandry across bushcricket taxa. This suggests that it is primarily non-sperm substances in the ejaculate itself, rather than the nuptial gift, that affect the receptivity of females to further matings (but see Sakaluk et al. 2006). This conclusion is supported by laboratory studies of individual bushcricket species in which the duration of ampulla attachment has been manipulated independently of the amount of spermatophylax consumed (Gwynne 1986; Wedell and Arak 1989; Simmons and Gwynne 1991). The precise nature of the ejaculate substances involved in manipulating female remating behavior in bushcrickets and the physiological mechanisms by which they operate, however, have yet to be determined (see Vahed 2006). The relative duration of the sexual refractory periods for males and females is expected to influence courtship roles (whether males or females are the more choosy or competitive sex when it comes to mating) via its effect on the operational sex ratio (reviewed in Gwynne 2001; see also Kokko and Monaghan 2001; Simmons and Kvarnemo 2006). Courtship role reversal has so far only been documented in bushcricket species with large spermatophores relative to male body mass (reviewed in Gwynne 2001). This may be because the production of relatively large ejaculates has a disproportionately higher cost compared with the production of small ejaculates. In the present study, a 10-fold increase in relative ampulla mass across species was associated with a 194-fold (antilog of 2.29) increase in the duration of the male’s sexual refractory period. Discussions of the role of male mating costs in causing courtship role reversal in bushcrickets have tended to focus on the costs to the male of producing large ‘‘nutritious’’ spermatophylaces or on the relative ‘‘parental’’ contribution of nutrients to offspring (reviewed in Simmons and Kvarnemo 2006). The present study, however, suggests that the production of the large ejaculate may be relatively more costly for the male than the production of the spermatophylax (the slope of the regression of log male refractory period against log ampulla mass is greater than the slope of log male refractory period against log spermatophylax mass). It should be noted that in the bushcricket K. nartee, which has become a model species for the study of the causes of courtship role reversal (reviewed in Gwynne 2001; Simmons and Kvarnemo 2006), the male produces one of the largest ejaculates, relative to male body mass, of any bushcricket studied so far. The ampulla is similar in mass to the spermatophylax, representing about 10% of male body mass (Table 1). It is possible, therefore, that it is the costs of producing the ejaculate, rather than the nuptial gift, that underlie cases of courtship role reversal in bushcrickets. Recently, there has been considerable debate over the extent to which females might benefit from mating with males with manipulative traits. Recent discussion has focused on the possible benefits to females in terms of producing sons that are better at manipulating females (Chapman et al. 2003a, 2003b; Cordero and Eberhard 2003, 2005; Eberhard and Cordero 2003; Stewart et al. 2005). The possible ‘‘good genes’’ benefits have received relatively less attention recently (but see Macias-Garcia and Ramirez 2005; Stuart-Fox 2005; Arnqvist 2006). If the disproportionately higher cost to males of producing a large spermatophore demonstrated across species in this study also occurs within species and if lower quality males bear a higher cost of producing a large spermatophore, spermatophore size could act as an honest indicator of male quality in species with large spermatophores (see Cordero 1995, 1996; Eberhard and Cordero 1995). Indeed, there is some evidence that spermatophore size reflects male quality in bushcrickets (see Jia et al. 2000; Lehmann GUC and Lehmann AW 2000a, 2000b, 2006; see also Engels and Sauer Vahed • A cost of manipulating females Lehmann GUC, Lehmann AW. 2000b. Spermatophore characteristics in bushcrickets vary with parasitism and remating interval. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 47:393–399. Lehmann GUC, Lehmann AW. 2006. Potential lifetime reproductive success of male bushcrickets parasitised by a phonotactic fly. Anim Behav. 71:1103–1110. Lessels CM. 2006. The evolutionary outcome of sexual conflict. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 361:301–317. Macias-Garcia CM, Ramirez E. 2005. Evidence that sensory traps can evolve into honest signals. Nature. 434:501–505. Naskrecki P. 2000. The phylogeny of katydids (Orthoptera: Ensifera: Tettigoniidae) and the evolution of their acoustic behaviour [PhD thesis]. Storrs, CT: University of Connecticut. Parker GA, Ball MA. 2005. Sperm competition, mating rate and the evolution of testis and ejaculate sizes: a population model. Biol Lett. 1:235–238. Purvis A, Rambaut A. 1995. Comparative analysis by independent contrasts (CAIC): an apple Macintosh application for analysing comparative data. Comput Appl Biosci. 11:247–251. Reinhold K, von Helversen D. 1997. Sperm number, spermatophore weight and remating in the bushcricket Poecilimon veluchianus. Ethology. 103:12–18. Rentz DCF, Colless DH. 1990. A classification of the shield-back katydids (Tettigoniinae) of the world. In: Bailey WJ, Rentz DCF, editors. The Tettigoniidae: biology, systematics and evolution. Bathurst (Australia): Crawford House Press. p. 352–377. Rice WR, Stewart AD, Morrow EH, Linder JE, Orteiza N, Byrne PG. 2006. Assessing sexual conflict in the Drosophila melanogaster laboratory model system. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 361:287–299. Sakaluk SK. 2000. Sensory exploitation as an evolutionary origin to nuptial food gifts in insects. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 267: 339–343. Sakaluk SK, Avery RL, Weddle CB. 2006. Cryptic sexual conflict in gift-giving insects: chasing the chase-away. Am Nat. 167:94–104. Sakaluk SK, Campbell MTH, Clark AP, Johnson JC, Keorpes PA. 2004. Hemolymph loss during nuptial feeding constrains male mating success in sagebrush crickets. Behav Ecol. 15:845–849. Simmons LW. 1990. Nuptial feeding in tettigoniids: male costs and rates of fecundity increase. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 27:43–47. Simmons LW. 1993. Some constraints on reproduction for male bushcrickets, Requena verticalis (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae)—diet, size and parasite load. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 32:135–139. Simmons LW. 1995. Male bushcrickets tailor spermatophores in relation to their remating intervals. Func Ecol. 9:881–886. Simmons LW. 2001. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press. Simmons LW. 2005. The evolution of polyandry: sperm competition, sperm selection and offspring viability. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 36:125–146. Simmons LW, Beesley L, Lindhjem P, Newbound D, Norris J, Wayne A. 1999. Nuptial feeding by male bushcrickets: an indicator of male quality? Behav Ecol. 10:263–269. Simmons LW, Craig M, Llorens T, Schinzig M, Hosken D. 1993. Bushcricket spermatophores vary in accord with sperm competition and parental investment theory. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 251: 183–186. Simmons LW, Gwynne DT. 1991. The refractory period of female katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae): sexual conflict over the remating interval? Behav Ecol. 2:276–282. Simmons LW, Kvarnemo C. 2006. Costs of breeding and their effects on the direction of sexual selection. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 273:465–470. Simmons LW, Teale RJ, Maier M, Standish RJ, Bailey WJ, Withers PC. 1992. Some costs of reproduction for male bushcrickets, Requena verticalis (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae): allocating resources to mate attraction and nuptial feeding. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 31:57–62. Stewart AD, Morrow EH, Rice WR. 2005. Assessing putative interlocus sexual conflict in Drosophila melanogaster using experimental evolution. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 272:2029–2035. Stuart-Fox D. 2005. Deception and the origin of honest signals. Trends Ecol Evol. 20:521–523. Thornhill R. 1976. Sexual selection and nuptial feeding behaviour in Bittacus apicalis (Insecta: Mecoptera). Am Nat. 110:529–548. Thornhill R. 1983. Cryptic female choice and its implications in the scorpionfly Harpobittacus nigriceps. Am Nat. 122:765–788. Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 30, 2015 Cordero C, Eberhard WG. 2005. Interaction between sexually antagonistic selection and mate choice in the evolution of female responses to male traits. Evol Ecol. 19:111–112. Davies PM, Dadour IR. 1989. A cost of mating by male Requena verticalis (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Ecol Entomol. 14:467–469. Eberhard WG, Cordero C. 1995. Sexual selection by cryptic female choice on male seminal products—a new bridge between sexual selection and reproductive physiology. Trends Ecol Evol. 10:493–496. Eberhard WG, Cordero C. 2003. Sexual conflict and female choice. Trends Ecol Evol. 18:438–439. Engelhardt V. 1915. On the structure of the alluring gland in Isophya acuminata Br-W. Bull Soc Entomol Moscow. 1:58–63. Engels S, Sauer KP. 2006. Resource-dependent nuptial feeding in Panorpa vulgaris: an honest signal for male quality. Behav Ecol. 17:628–632. Gavrilets S, Arnqvist G, Friberg U. 2001. The evolution of female mate choice by sexual conflict. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 268: 531–539. Gillott C. 2003. Male accessory gland secretions: modulators of female reproductive physiology and behavior. Ann Rev Entomol. 48: 163–184. Gwynne DT. 1986. Courtship feeding in katydids: investment in offspring or in obtaining fertilisations? Am Nat. 128:342–352. Gwynne DT. 1990. Testing parental investment and the control of sexual selection in katydids: the operational sex ratio. Am Nat. 136: 474–484. Gwynne DT. 2001. Katydids and bushcrickets: reproductive behaviour and evolution of the Tettigoniidae. Ithaca (NY): Cornell University Press. Härdling R, Smith HG. 2005. Antagonistic coevolution under sexual conflict. Evol Ecol. 19:137–150. Hartley JC. 1990. Egg biology of the Tettigonidae. In: Bailey WJ, Rentz DCF, editors. The Tettigoniidae: biology, systematics and evolution. Bathurst (Australia): Crawford House Press. p. 41–70. Hartley JC. 1991. The use of sound by Leptophyes laticauda. Metaleptea. 13(1):4–6. Hartley JC, Dean RL. 1974. Ephippiger cruciger as a laboratory insect (Orthoptera: tettigoniidae). J Nat Hist. 8:349–354. Harvey PH, Pagel MD. 1991. The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford (UK): Oxford University Press. Hayashi F. 1993. Male mating costs in two insect species (Protohermes, Megaloptera) that produce large spermatophores. Anim Behav. 45:343–349. Heller K-G. 1984. Zur Bioakustic und phylogenie der gattung Poecilimon (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae, Phaneropterinae). Zool Jb Syst. 111:69–117. Heller K-G. 1992. Risk shift between males and females in the pair-forming behaviour of bushcrickets. Naturwissenschaften. 79: 89–91. Heller K-G. 2006. Song evolution and speciation in bushcrickets. In: Drosopoulos S, Claridge MF, editors. Insect sounds and communication: physiology, behaviour, ecology and evolution. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press. p. 137–152. Heller KG, Faltin S, Fleischmann P, von Helveren O. 1998. The chemical composition of the spermatophore in some species of phaneropterid bushcrickets (Orthoptera: Tettigonioidea). J Insect Physiol. 44:1001–1008. Heller KG, von Helversen D. 1991. Operational sex ratio and individual mating frequencies in two bushcricket species (Orthoptera, Tettigonioidea, Poecilimon) Ethology. 89:211–228. Hosken DJ, Snook R. 2005. How important is sexual conflict? Am Nat. 165:S1–S4. Hosken DJ, Stockley P. 2003. Benefits of polyandry: a life history perspective. Evol Biol. 33:173–194. Jia Z, Jiang Z, Sakaluk SK. 2000. Nutritional condition influences investment by male katydids in nuptial food gifts. Ecol Entomol. 25:115–118. Kokko H, Monaghan P. 2001. Predicting the direction of sexual selection. Ecol Lett. 4:159–165. Kondoh M. 2001. Co-evolution of nuptial gift and female multiple mating resulting in diverse breeding systems. Evol Ecol Res. 3:75–89. Lehmann GUC, Lehmann AW. 2000a. Female bushcrickets mated with parasitised males show rapid remating and reduced fecundity (Orthoptera: Phaneropteridae: Poecilimon mariannae). Naturwissenschaften. 87:404–407. 505 506 bushcrickets (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae). Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 263:1257–1265. Wedell N. 1993a. Mating effort or paternal investment? Incorporation rate and cost of male donations in the wartbiter. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 32:239–246. Wedell N. 1993b. Spermatophore size in bushcrickets: comparative evidence for nuptial gifts as a sperm protection device. Evolution. 47:1203–1212. Wedell N. 1994. Dual function of the bushcricket spermatophore. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 258:181–185. Wedell N. 2005. Female receptivity in butterflies and moths. J Exp Biol. 208:3433–3440. Wedell N, Arak A. 1989. The wartbiter spermatophore and its effect on female reproductive output (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae, Decticus verrucivorus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 24:117–125. Wedell N, Gage JG, Parker G. 2002. Sperm competition, male prudence and sperm-limited females. Trends Ecol Evol. 17: 313–320. Westlake KP, Rowe L. 1999. Developmental costs of male sexual traits in the water strider Rheumatobates rileyi. Can J Zool. 77:917– 922. Wolfner MF. 2002. The gifts that keep on giving: physiological functions and evolutionary dynamics of male seminal proteins in Drosophila. Heredity. 88:85–93. Downloaded from http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on September 30, 2015 Torres-Vila LM, Stockel J, Rodriguez-Molina MC. 1997. Physiological factors regulating polyandry in lobesia botrana (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae). Physiol Entomol. 22:387–393. Tregenza T, Wedell N, Chapman T. 2006. Introduction. sexual conflict: a new paradigm? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 361:229–234. Vahed K. 1997. Copulation and spermatophores in the Ephippigerinae (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae): prolonged copulation is associated with a smaller nuptial gift in Uromenus rugosicollis. Serv J Orthop Res. 6:83–89. Vahed K. 1998a. Sperm precedence and the potential of the nuptial gift to function as paternal investment in the tettigoniid Steropleurus stali Bolivar (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae: Ephippigerinae). J Orthop Res. 7:223–227. Vahed K. 1998b. The function of nuptial feeding in insects: a review of empirical studies. Biol Rev. 73:43–78. Vahed K. 2002. Coercive copulation in the alpine bushcricket Anonconotus alpinus Yersin (Tettigoniidae: Tettigoniinae: Platycleidini). Ethology. 108:1065–1075. Vahed K. 2006. Larger ejaculate volumes are associated with a lower degree of polyandry across bushcricket taxa. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 273:2387–2394. Vahed K. 2007. All that glisters is not gold: sensory bias, sexual conflict and nuptial feeding in insects and spiders. Ethology. 113:105–127. Vahed K, Gilbert FS. 1996. Differences across taxa in nuptial gift size correlate with differences in sperm number and ejaculate volume in Behavioral Ecology
Keep reading this paper — and 50 million others — with a free Academia account
Used by leading Academics
Richard Cloutier
Université du Québec à Rimouski
Billie J. Swalla
University of Washington
Dirk K F Meijer
University of Groningen
Yasha Hartberg
Texas A&M University