Alternative to Inglehart's Values Constructs
Zoltán Lakatos
Budapest University of Technology and Economics
Working paper
6.488 words (excluding figures, tables, references, appendices, and abstract (153 words))
Abstract
Despite abundant criticism regarding their theoretical foundations and composition,
Inglehart's values constructs have become near currencies in cross-cultural research. Drawing
on these critiques, this paper argues that Inglehart's three most influential indicators of
cultural values: the Postmaterialism Index, the self-expression-survival, and the seculartraditional measures obfuscate the complexity of the value space at the individual level. To
overcome the inherent limitations of Inglehart's instruments I present an alternative approach
in the form of multiple correspondence analysis (MCA), a geometrical approach whose
theoretical foundations are in field analysis. A recently developed variant of MCA performing
rotation separates out those axes of cultural values (religiosity, authoritarianism, materialism,
and achievement orientation) that Inglehart's scales treat as part of broader cultural
dimensions. The results corroborate findings by other critics of Inglehart's methods
suggesting that the failure to identify the libertarian-authoritarian axis as separate from both
religiosity and materialism stems from a mechanistic view of the culture-economy dialectic.
1
An earlier version of this paper was presented at the R User conference in July 2012 in
Bordeaux, France and the RC33 Conference of the International Sociological Association in
Sydney, Australia in July, 2012.
Acknowledgment
Throughout this work I have benefited from collaboration with Marie Chavent and
Michel van de Velden, developers of the software packages PCAmixdata for R and CAR for
MatLAB respectively. Without their extensive support the statistical analyses applying a
recently developed variant of multiple correspondence analysis would not have been possible.
I also thank Róbert Tardos, Dominique Joye, Jörg Blasius, Nikosz Fokasz, Béla Janky, Zoltán
Kmetty, János Balázs Kocsis, Ákos Kopper, and Frank T. Zsigo for their helpful comments
and suggestions. I am solely responsible for any error and omission.
Funding
This work was supported by the Department of Sociology and Communication at the
Budapest University of Technology and Economics (BME).
2
Postmaterialist Shift: Theses and Measures
Empirical sociology's interest in cultural change goes back to the early history of social
science. Over the past few decades, thanks to the evolution of information technology,
representative surveys have been providing a growing body of cultural indicators aimed at
measuring these processes. The World Values Survey (WVS), initiated by political scientist
Ronald Inglehart stands out in thematic scope and geographical coverage among these
programs. Drawing heavily on findings from the WVS, Inglehart's theses on and indicators of
cultural change are of particular interest as they have profoundly influenced scholarly debates
on the content of cultural values and their interconnections with various economic and
political processes. Of Inglehart's three most influential measures, the Postmaterialism Index
is a key indicator of the cultural shift presented in his early theses (Inglehart 1971, 1977)
while the other two, the self-expression-survival and the secular-traditional values constructs
(Inglehart 1997; Inglehart and Baker 2000) aim to tap a more complex set of phenomena. The
latter two instruments reflect the increasing sophistication Inglehart's methodology has
undergone, partly in response to the debates on his original theses. Nevertheless, as both the
core of his theses and his approach to measuring values have remained constant, concerns
with these three constructs are remarkably similar.
Without undue simplification, the theoretical foundations of these measures comprise
two complementary hypotheses. The scarcity thesis, borrowed from Maslow's needs theory
(1970) posits that people value anything that is in short supply, therefore so long as physical
survival and material security are not taken for granted, their value system is dominated by
materialistic aspirations. An equally important aspect is the role attributed to religiosity and
3
authoritarianism, identified as "traditional values" and equally associated with economic
scarcity and physical insecurity (Inglehart and Baker 2000). Increasing affluence leads to a
de-emphasis of not only material gains but also religion and unquestioned authority in favor
of secular, libertarian values, and "self-expression". The socialization thesis conceives of
value orientations acquired during one's formative years as stable over adulthood. Periodical
fluctuations, that is decreasing or increasing materialism within a given age cohort may occur
as a result of mainly economic booms or downturns, but generations raised in significantly
different material conditions retain their different values.
Central to the empirical evidence behind the Postmaterialism Index is a factor analysis
of responses to questions on national priorities (Table 1). Table 2 shows how each of the
original 12 items load onto a single component. Given that most of these questions address
policy preferences rather than values, the relationship posited by Inglehart between the private
and the public domain is problematic. Flanagan (1982a, 1982b; Inglehart and Flanagan 1987)
has argued that the Postmaterialism Index is an inadequate measure of two separate
dimensions of political conflicts in Western societies: an emphasis on economic versus noneconomic issues and preference for libertarian, as opposed to authoritarian policies. The
Postmaterialism Index classifies as "materialists" those who are simply authoritarians but see
non-economic policy issues as paramount. The problematic inference of values from answers
to questions on public policy is also discussed at length in Marsh (1975), Lafferty and
Knutsen (1985), Trump (1991), Haller (2002), and Majima and Savage (2007).
[Table 1 and Table 2 about here]
Several authors have also pointed out that the evolution of the Postmaterialism Index
does not follow the trajectory posited by Inglehart's theses. Growing portions of materialists
4
among the young in a number of European countries (Böltken and Jagodzinski 1985), the
absence of significant effect of formative economic experiences on values in adulthood (Duch
and Taylor 1993, 1994; De Graaf and Evans 1996; Clarke and Dutt 1991; Clarke, Dutt, and
Rapkin 1997a, 1997b), and high instability, attributed specifically to random distribution of
responses and non-attitudes (Van Deth 1983; Davis and Davenport 1999; Davis, Dowley, and
Silver 1999) call into question the validity of the instrument, the underlying hypotheses or
both. These problems are in part related to Inglehart's reliance on aggregated data for
multivariate analyses. The factor analyses whose outputs he presents as evidence of a
materialist-postmaterialist axis were performed on country averages. Inglehart justifies
aggregation by the elimination of measurement error, arguing that the aggregate pattern is
determined by those who "give «real» responses [because] random variations tend to cancel
each other out" (Inglehart 1983: 89), an argument also advanced with regard to some of his
later studies. However, aggregate consistence of factors and stability of indicators derived
from aggregates may conceal inconsistence and instability at the individual level (Van Deth
1983; Davis et al.; Haller 2002; Majima and Savage 2007). This is a case of disaggregation
error or ecological fallacy whereby individual attributes are inferred from macro indicators
overlooking complexity at the micro level.
With the WVS’s geographical coverage expanding and the availability of longitudinal
data, Inglehart has constructed a more comprehensive series of indicators that he claims to
account for much of the value change taking place across societies with very different cultural
traditions (Inglehart 1997). The most important of his new measures are the traditionalsecular/rational and the survival-self-expression/well-being axes (Figure 1, Table 3) that
define a two-dimensional value space in which societies form clusters that appear to represent
historical-cultural entities in a "Global Cultural Map" (Figure 2). While the pool of variables
used for this analysis is different from the policy preference questions that form the basis of
5
the Postmaterialism Index, their use is no less problematic. First, as with the Postmaterialism
Index, Inglehart initially derived these scores from factor analysis performed on national
aggregates, using country averages.1 Second, the composition of the resulting value
dimensions exposes a reductionist approach similar to the construction of the Postmaterialism
Index. Most importantly, both dimensions are too heterogeneous constructs to be considered
basic value orientations. The traditional-secular/rational dimension ― in addition to the
confusion about the labeling of its poles ― combines items tapping religiosity with indicators
of authoritarianism and achievement values as if these were indicators of one single
underlying dimension. The objection can be made ― and I will test the hypothesis below ―
that this is not the case, i.e., the impact of non-authoritarian religiosity and authoritarian
secularism is too important for religiosity and authoritarianism to be collapsed under one
single construct. Of the variables from which the survival-well-being dimension were
originally extracted, six ("happiness", "life satisfaction", "sate of health", "trust in people",
"having [a sense of] free choice", and Affect Balance, an index of "positive versus negative"
feelings) tap not values but psychological states. A seventh variable associated with this
dimension is the Postmaterialism Index ("priority to economic and physical security over selfexpression and quality of life"), whose construction is problematic for the reasons reviewed
above. It seems legitimate to suppose that had the psychographic variables and the
Postmaterialism Index and the other non-values been omitted from Inglehart’s analysis, a
much different configuration would have emerged from even a two-dimension solution.
Obviously, Inglehart worked out the cultural map to remedy the major flaws of the
Postmaterialism Index, partly in response to criticism of his postmaterialism theses. Still, he
seems to have followed the same path by treating preferences as indicators of deep-seated
1
This remark concerns the first version of the two-dimensional cultural map (Inglehart 1997: 82) but the same issues persist
with regard to the second version (Inglehart and Baker 2000), a reduced, 10-item battery backed by a factor analysis at the
individual as well as the aggregate level. See remarks for Table 3.
6
orientations, combining qualitatively distinct orientations into one single dimension
(libertarian and economic concerns under the postmaterialist label and later under "selfexpression", authoritarianism and religiosity under "traditional values").
[Figure 1, Table 3 and Figure 2 about here]
Finally, it is remarkable that equivalence issues rarely come up in the debates on
Inglehart's measures (Haller 2002). One such exception is a study by van De Vijver and
Poortinga (2002) in which the authors tested the equivalence of the Postmaterialism Index
and found that it is configurally invariant at the individual level in all but one of the 39
regions examined but added that the construct becomes more salient in affluent countries.
However, they also concluded that in order to be comparable across countries, some items
have to be eliminated. Such issues may eventually compromise the comparability of the
secular-traditional and the self-expression-survival scales, although findings of a
comprehensive test (involving not only configural, but also metric and scalar invariance) are
yet to be made available.2
Multiple Correspondence Analysis
A sensible alternative to Inglehart's instruments has to, at a minimum (1) build on items
that are actual values, (2) focus on the individual level, and (3) avoid ambiguity in the
resulting constructs. In addition, (4) it should restrict cross-cultural comparisons to (country)
scores satisfying scalar invariance. The choice of multiple correspondence analysis (MCA)
2
In comparison, the Schwartz value scales have been subjected to a comprehensive test of invariance and were found to
satisfy configural and metric (but not full) equivalence in 20 countries (Davidov, Schmidt, and Schwartz 2008).
7
for the study presented in this section follows from the first three considerations. Given that
equivalence testing of summary indicators provided by MCA is still mostly unexplored
territory, the fourth requirement is a direction for future research.
Although its empirical potential is amply demonstrated thanks notably to Bourdieu's
work on taste and cultural stratification (1979), correspondence analysis is still rarely applied
in sociological studies. A geometric data analysis (GDA) method using an observationsproperties table, MCA does not impose any constraint on the data (Blasius and Greenacre
2006), unlike factor analysis which assumes linear constraints. MCA does not require the
averages for the variables whose structure it seeks to reveal and thus allows the researcher
more freedom in identifying latent structures based on how categories of the analyzed
variables are plotted in an n-dimensional space. Given that many of Inglehart's factor analyses
are performed on national aggregates, linear constraint was not a concern in his studies but
since his variables are categorical, scale properties become an issue once his factor analyses
are performed at the individual level.3 Another consideration in favor of MCA is its focus on
relations rather than variables: instead of looking for combinations of independent variables
that might explain a priori dependent variables, "MCA starts from and carefully constructs
that which is to be explained" (Le Roux et al. 2008: 1053). The power of correspondence
analysis lies in its representation of that complexity at two levels: a geometrical space defined
by the relations between the properties (in this case values) and a space where the units of
observations (individuals) are located (Rouanet, Ackermann, and Le Roux 2000). The closer
individuals are positioned within that space, the more properties they share, and inversely.
This mirrors Bourdieu's meta-theoretical framework which "identifies reality not with
3
In addition, many of Inglehart's items have dissimilar scales, from binary to 4- or 10-point Likert-type, etc. Such scale
inconsistencies render the application of multivariate methods using linear methods problematic.
8
substances but with relations" (Bourdieu 1987: 150, emphasis added), a relational way of
thinking that Bourdieu traces back to the origins of modern science.4
The substantialist framework is evident in many of Inglehart's writings, most
significantly in his adoption of a methodological apparatus that is reminiscent of the
"sociology of variables", a perspective that Bourdieu repeatedly criticized for its mechanistic
approach and reductionism (Rouanet et al. 2008). The focus on the economic "origins" of
postmaterialism and other values, laid out in Inglehart's scarcity and socialization hypotheses
as well as their later, refined versions (e.g., the emphasis on "individual resources" in the
theory of human development, (Welzel, Inglehart, and Kligemann 2003; Welzel 2002) is one
example of that deductive logic. Another is the opposite perspective, also adopted by
Inglehart in his later work on the macroeconomic "effects" of cultural values (Granato,
Inglehart, and Leblang 1996a, 1996b) where he and his coauthors argue that based on the
endogenous growth model proposed by economists (e.g., Lucas 1988; Romer 1990) to
overcome the limitations of neoclassical theories of economic growth (Solow 1956; Swan
1956), a cultural or value component is assumed to have an independent impact on
productivity and thus to contribute to economic growth. The latter argument is presented with
reference to the dominant interpretation of Weber's Protestant ethic thesis (Weber, Baehr, and
Wells 2002) which, according to Granato et al. suggests an independent effect of culture on
economic outcomes. However, as recent scholarship on the Protestant ethic thesis has shown,
Weber's views on the interconnections between cultural and economic phenomena are not
what is generally assumed by their dominant interpretations as Weber never proposed a
theory of "causation" by cultural values. Instead, he conceived of the relationship between
culture and the economy as "elective affinities" (Howe 1978; Chalcraft and Harrington 2001;
4
For a general discussion of the bourdieusian conception of social space with implications for CA, see Wacquant (2013).
9
Löwy 2004; Chalcraft 2005; Runciman 2005; Treviño 2005; McKinnon 2010), an insight that
informed Bourdieu's concept of field and habitus (Bourdieu 1980: 34).
In short, Inglehart's scholarship treats values once as outcomes, once as causes of macro
(mainly economic) phenomena. However, none of these accounts grasps the complexity of
how values are embedded in the socio-economic context. Social contexts are fields where
structure and agency, material and symbolic, economic and cultural are inextricably bound
together in ways irreducible to a "dominant effect" or causality. Values share with Bourdieu's
habitus the property of being a modus operandi, a generating formula of action, a system of
stable dispositions that is both structured and structuring (Bourdieu 1980: 88). The space of
values is a field where agents occupy different positions according to their own "normative
emphases that underlie and justify the functioning of social institutions" (Schwartz 2011: 314,
emphasis added) ― where the terms "underlie" and "justify" capture both the materiality and
the symbolic aspects of values "put to work". Such a perspective "translates the question of
'why' into a 'how' that is answerable in terms of a situation and its typal vocabulary of
motives" (Mills 1940: 906). The theoretical framework in which correspondence analysis
finds its empirical utility is therefore a departure from both the rigid structuralist and the
substantialist methods of inquiry. MCA is thus the method of choice in field analysis.
Overcoming the limitations of competing theoretical traditions around the "rather formulaic
fractal divide between the macro and micro, society and individual, material and social"
(Savage and Silva 2013: 124), field analysis ― and by implication, MCA ― concentrate on
that which the sociology of variables ignores: relations.5
In addition to Bourdieu's wide-ranging applications (besides his now classic
"Distinction" (1979), these include Bourdieu and De Saint Martin 1978; De Saint Martin and
Bourdieu 1987; Bourdieu 1999, 2000), the empirical relevance of MCA to the actor-field
5
For a discussion of field theory in the social sciences, see Martin 2003.
10
approach is supported by a number of recent studies on taste and lifestyles (Le Roux et al.
2008; Silva and Wright 2009; Blasius and Mühlichen 2010; Bennett, Bustamante, and Frow
2013; Silva and Le Roux 2011; Hanquinet 2013; Hanquinet, Roose, and Savage 2013;
Purhonen and Wright 2013), fields of power (Hjellbrekke et al. 2007; Lebaron and Le Roux
2011; Flemmen 2012; Buhlmann, David, and Mach 2013; Kropp 2013), class and social
inequalities (Veenstra 2007), and political attitudes (Harrits et al. 2010). Most relevant to our
subject however is a study on the British value system by Majima and Savage (2007) that, in
addition to applying MCA because it provides a representation of the complexity of
"individual value spaces" is also a critical engagement with Inglehart's work. Crucially from
our point of view, Majima and Savage conclude that "the supposed shift towards postmaterialist values that Inglehart claims for the British case, as for other countries, is in fact an
artefact of questionnaire design" (ibid: 312). In the following I extend on their analysis.
The Cloud of Modalities
Unrotated Solution
I use data from the first four waves of the WVS, collected between 1981 and 2004 in 84
countries, a total sample of 267.870 (World Values Survey Association 2009). The choice of
variables is an aspect where constraints follow from the fact that only a smaller set of
questions were asked in all four waves. The 28 questions selected and combined into 19
variables that were used in the analysis reflect a compromise between the requirements
stemming from the criticisms reviewed above (i.e., the questions should tap values, not policy
preferences or mental states), and the availability of variables across four waves. The original
11
responses were recoded into binary modalities (see Table 5 in the Appendix for recoding
rules).6
[Figure 3 about here]
Figure 3 shows the first four axes emerging from the unrotated solution of our 19
variables. At first inspection, the cloud of modalities defining the first two axes looks similar
to Inglehart's 2-dimensional value space presented in the Global Cultural Map in Figure 2.
The first dimension (horizontal in upper plane) translates an opposition between values
associated with traditional morality (on the right) and their opposite (on the left). Traditional
morality consists of strong religiosity and authoritarianism which stand in opposition to
secular and libertarian values. The second (vertical) axis represents an opposition between
achievement orientation (bottom) and materialistic attitudes (top).7 In addition, modalities
with higher than average contribution to Axis 2 include some items associated with
authoritarianism and libertarianism. Axis 2 therefore looks remarkably similar to Inglehart's
self-expression-survival scale. The third dimension (horizontal, in bottom plane) captures an
opposition between on one hand an attitude that is both achievement-oriented and
materialistic8 and on the other its opposite, an attitude that values neither achievement nor
material gain. This latter pole also includes a rejection of outgroups. The fourth (vertical) axis
translates a conflict between an achievement orientation that is not materialistic and a
materialistic outlook lacking any sense of achievement.
6
The only exception is the frequency of participating in religious services, recoded in three categories.
For ease of interpretation, the maps do not show those modalities whose contribution to the axes is lower than the average,
defined as dividing 100 by the total of modalities. The contributions of the modalities are included in Table 4 in the
Appendix.
8
As shown by the high contribution of the modalities "achievement most important" and "good pay most important".
7
12
The semantic heterogeneity of the four axes (religiosity combined with
authoritarianism, materialism with achievement values, etc.) makes this rendering of the
value space difficult to interpret. At the same time, alternative axes emerge in both diagrams.
With Plane 3 and 4, an alternative axis running from the top right to the bottom left quadrant
is a more straightforward opposition because it simply reflects the importance attached to
achievement without the interference of the items related to materialism. Likewise, another
line linking the top left to the bottom right quadrant is a better representation of materialism
than Axes 3 or 4. The same opportunity for simplification is apparent with regard to Axes 1
and 2. A line running from the bottom right to the top left quadrant captures religiosity
without the influence of the modalities related to authoritarianism. And vice versa, linking the
bottom left and the top right quadrants is another line tapping authoritarianism without
religiosity. These distributions suggest that the rotation of the axes would yield semantically
more homogeneous dimensions. In "MCA as we know it" such alternative axes often make
more sense than the original ones, yet these cannot be provided in most current statistical
packages.9 This is not necessarily an inconvenience when summary indicators are not sought
― but becomes a hindrance once the aim of the study is to also provide such indicators.
Rotated Solution
Rotation in multiple correspondence analysis has been proposed by a number of authors
(Kiers 1991; Adachi 2004; Velden and Kiers 2005). Kiers's idea was to extend the varimax
criterion to arrive at simple structures in PCAMIX, which is a principal component method
for a mixture of qualitative and quantitative variables. The varimax criterion is expressed
9
Discussing the configuration of modalities in the 3-axis solution of their MCA of 21 variables from the WVS, Majima and
Savage also conclude that "rather than (…) endorsing the centrality of the materialist-post-materialist (or 'survival-selfexpressive') dimension, it [i.e., their MCA solution] suggests ruptures between libertarian and authoritarian views" (2007:
305). Nevertheless, their MCA only provides an unrotated solution where the corresponding items emerge counter-posed
along a virtual axis (linking the top-right and the bottom-left quadrants, see Figure 2 in Majima and Savage 2007: 306).
13
with squared loadings defined as correlation ratios for qualitative and squared correlations for
quantitative variables. Later formulations (Saracco, Chavent, and Kuentz 2010) use a singular
value decomposition (SVD) approach to produce the component scores, squared loadings and
the principal coordinates of the categories. This procedure searches for an optimal rotation
using the iterative procedure suggested by Kaiser (1958) for principal component analysis. It
leads to the same rotated solution as Kiers’ method, but is computationally more efficient.
Until very recently, these techniques were not available in statistical software. The first
available are CAR (Correspondence Analysis with Rotation), under MATLAB developed by
Lorenzo-Seva, van de Velden, and Kiers (2009), and PCAMIXdata, a package for R by
Saracco et al. (2010). The rotated solutions presented in this section were obtained with these
two packages.10
[Figure 4 about here]
We saw that authoritarianism and religiosity one hand, and achievement orientation and
materialism on the other would not separate out if we were to retain the axes provided by the
unrotated MCA solution. In this respect, the unrotated solution is reminiscent of Inglehart's
factor analyses where these domains are subsumed under the more comprehensive seculartraditional and self-expression-survival constructs. However in the rotated 4-axis solution
presented in Figure 4, the new axes are aligned precisely along those lines that appear as a
sensible alternative to the original (unrotated) axes. Here religiosity, authoritarianism,
materialism and achievement values (Axes 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively) constitute
unambiguously separate dimensions. Compared with the unrotated solution, the percentage of
10
The two packages were used complementarily. The exploratory analyses were performed using CAR, based on the
summary Burt table. CAR also calculated the contribution of modalities to the axes. PCAmixdata provided the coordinates
at the individual level for the retained unrotated and rotated solutions. The results presented below come from an MCA with
orthogonal rotation.
14
variance (inertia) explained by the first dimension dropped significantly (from 27.1 to 16.3%)
while that of the second dimension increased (from 6.4 to 9.9%). There were also increases in
the variances explained by the third and the fourth dimensions (from 4.9 to 7.8%, and from
3.1 to 7.6%). This redistribution of the variances indicates that the axes resulting from the
rotated solution are semantically more coherent than the more ambiguous scales in the
unrotated solution. The most spectacular gain in consistence is due to the picking up of parts
of the inertia from the first unrotated dimension by Axes 2 to 4 in the rotated solution. Also,
while items related to materialism and achievement appear as items with significant
contribution also in Plane 1-2 of the unrotated solution, they are of no significance to the first
two dimensions in the rotated solution, religiosity and materialism. The same can be said of a
couple of items related to authoritarianism and libertarianism (e.g., acceptance and rejection
of divorce and single mothers) that in the unrotated solution have significant contributions to
Axes 3 and 4 but are not relevant to the third and fourth dimensions in the rotated solution,
capturing materialism and achievement values in a more straightforward fashion. The
resulting increase in homogeneity has led to scales that are easier to interpret and thus provide
a more plausible description of the value space than the semantically less consistent axes in
Inglehart's Global Cultural Map.
We saw that Inglehart conceives of materialism as part of a broader domain that
includes also "traditional" and "survival" values. The emergence of materialism as a compact
construct in the rotated 4-axis substantiates the various criticisms addressed to the
postmaterialism theses, reviewed above. One could object that Inglehart's typology is based
on a minimalist configuration and other dimensions would emerge were we to carry his
analysis beyond just two dimensions. Indeed, Inglehart readily admits that we would get a
"somewhat different solution" if further dimensions were retained in his factor analyses. Still,
he dismisses that suggestion as misleading by arguing that "(d)oing so produces a far more
15
complicated result that might superficially seem more scholarly" whereas "cross-cultural
variation (…) can be interpreted with a relatively parsimonious model" (Inglehart 1997: 91).
However, when techniques adapted to categorical data and the complexity of the individual
level such as MCA are applied carefully, the result is not more but less complicated latent
constructs. If anything, it is his constructs that are unnecessarily complex because they
obfuscate rather than clarify the structure of values and the direction of cultural change. The
breaking down of the 19 variables into four semantically distinct dimensions at the individual
level is an indication of substantive differences in the content of these constructs, rather than
an artifact of "scholarly sophistication".
The axes resulting from the rotated MCA solution translate values that are
incommensurable and thus should be kept separate. They stand as empirical support to the
core of Inglehart criticisms. Inglehart's summary indicators are indeed reductionist: religiosity
is not a corollary of authoritarianism, it is therefore wrong to combine these two distinct
values in a secular-traditional construct. A sizeable portion of the sample is religious and
libertarian or secular and authoritarian ― dispositions that the two-dimensional Global
Cultural Map fails to discern. Likewise, materialism differs from and should not be conflated
with authoritarianism, religiosity, and low priority to achievement. Whereas in Inglehart's
typology, materialism entails a complex set of attitudes, actual materialism, as measured by
Axis 3 in the rotated solution is (really) only about the importance of material gain and
nothing else ― and its opposite value is not post- but non-materialism.
[Table 4 about here]
16
The Cloud of Individuals
Given the scarcity of scholarship on the techniques exploring the equivalence of
constructs resulting from MCA and related methods,11 any comparison of scores produced by
the solutions presented in the previous section has to be treated with caution. For this reason,
this paper leaves the discussion of country scores to further studies, probably until advances
in the comparability of MCA solutions are made. With that qualification, an overview rather
than detailed country comparisons may reasonably complement the findings related to the
cloud of modalities.
Landmark Countries and Individuals
Studies using MCA sometimes describe landmark individuals in order to make better
sense of the clouds (e.g., Le Roux et al. 2008; Purhonen and Wright 2013). The four
landmark cases presented in Figure 5 have been selected to represent "typical" individuals
from four national samples that stand also as "typical" in the sense that the distribution of
respondents in their respective clouds is illustrative of easy-to-interpret value configurations.
This is because the majority in these clouds are clustered further away from the origin than in
any other national samples. "Typical" here refers to ideal types in the Weberian sense, i.e., an
accentuation of certain characteristics (Hendricks and Breckinridge Peters 1973) of more or
less homogeneous clusters of national samples and should be understood as "caricatures"
rather than country medians.
The Jordanian sample (collected in 2001) is characteristic of a premodern cultural
profile: mostly religious, authoritarian, non-materialist and not achievement-oriented. The
majority in the Russian sample (1995) display a value system typical of societies marked by
several decades of communist rule: secular, authoritarian, materialist and not achievement11
The few exceptions include Blasius and Thiessen (2006a, 2006b) and Blasius and Graeff (2009).
17
oriented. The Swedish sample (1999) is a showcase of late modern societies: mostly secular
and libertarian, while also achievement-oriented but non-materialistic. The Colombian
sample (1998) is for the most part religious, libertarian, but also materialistic and
achievement-oriented. In addition to being ideal types, these four national clouds are
significant for a further consideration: two of them represent configurations that do not fit
Inglehart's typology. The Jordanian sample, while authoritarian, is not materialistic, and the
majority of Colombians, while strongly religious, are not authoritarian. Only the Swedish and
the Russian clouds conform to the combinations "allowed" by Inglehart's Global Cultural
Map: secular and libertarian, non-materialist and achievement oriented, and vice versa. As
their respective positions in the maps suggest, the landmark cases correspond to individuals
with different cultural values. More interesting, however, are their similarities and differences
regarding variables that are not included in the clouds of modalities as these are instructive of
the maps’ explanatory potential. The following description focuses on this latter category.
[Figure 5 about here]
Secular and authoritarian [top-left], materialist and not achievement-oriented
[bottom-right]
Case 122 955, "Svetlana" is a Russian woman aged 70 (surveyed in 1995) living alone
in a big city in Central Russia. She has no sense of freedom and control in her life and is
generally distrustful of others. Very dissatisfied with the people in national office, she thinks
that Russia is run by a few big interests. She finds that compared to 10 years before (i.e.,
under communism), a larger share of Russians live in poverty, which she attributes to unfair
conditions in her country. For Svetlana, a democratic system is fundamentally bad ―
probably because of her own experiences of what goes under the label of democracy, rather
18
than out of a rejection of democracy itself ―, a stance that explains her preference for
military rule and a strong leader. Overall, she says that Russia's political system was better
under the communist regime, and would vote for a small nationalist party.
Secular and libertarian [bottom-left], non-materialist and achievement-oriented [topleft]
Case 189 046, "Magnus" is a married man aged 61 (surveyed in 1999), living in a
household of two in a medium-size city in Southern Sweden. In the past he formally belonged
to a Protestant denomination, but qualifies himself as "not religious". He is very satisfied with
his life, albeit with a limited sense of freedom and choice. Generally trustful of others,
Magnus is a member in several organizations and associations, and has done voluntary unpaid
work. He thinks that people, rather than the government should take more responsibility to
ensure their own well-being and views competition as good, rather than bad. At the same
time, he favors environmental legislation, and even agrees with tax increases to prevent
environmental pollution. Regarding the political system, Magnus rates Sweden’s as rather
good, and would vote for a centrist liberal party.
Religious and libertarian [bottom-right], materialist and achievement-oriented [topright]
Case 104 551, "Isabel" is a 25-year old (surveyed in 1998) childless single woman
living with her parents in a Central Colombian city. She is both very religious and antiauthoritarian, an attitude that is not in short supply among Colombians. She is accepting
toward childless women, divorce and outgroups, and has participated in subversive political
action. Satisfied with her financial situation as well as with her life in general, she feels a
great deal of freedom and control but is distrustful of others. Regarding attitudes toward
19
government and politics, Isabel's support for Unión Patriótica, a socialist party is probably
related to her assessment that Colombia is run by a few big interests and poverty is on the rise
as well as her preference for increased government ownership. However, Isabel’s preference
for radical change is confined within democratic limits as she rejects alternatives by the
military or a strong man.
Religious and authoritarian [top-right], non-materialist and not achievement-oriented
[bottom-left]
Case 239 119, "Rashid" is a 28-year old single, childless man (born 1973) living with
his parents in a small town in North-West Jordan. He has completed high school and is
employed as a non-manual office worker. Rashid attends mosque several times a week and is
for the unconditional respect of parents, and thinks that men make better political leaders than
women do. He defines himself as a Muslim above all (i.e., before being Jordanian or Arab,
etc.), and has also a strong clan identity. Satisfied with his life, he is totally disengaged from
politics, would not vote for any party and prefers people with strong religious beliefs in public
office.
Discussing landmark individuals from two separate MCAs resulting from a British and
a Finnish survey on cultural taste, Purhonen and Wright note that "(e)ven as we cannot claim
that any one individual is typical or representative of their position, nor can we dismiss the
meaningfulness of their account of that position as some sort of empirical reality" (2013:
265). The same proposition is warranted here with the addition that while the properties
discussed above that were not included in the MCA obviously cannot be fully accounted for
by the positions that the four landmark individuals occupy in the clouds, many of them "make
sense" in light of the corresponding individual values. It would be a mistake to dismiss such
20
similarities and dissimilarities as merely anecdotal. The positions occupied by individuals in
the clouds are meaningful beyond the variables used in the MCA.
Relationship with macro variables
The rationale for better measures is to also better understand how cultural values are
related to various macro phenomena. Because the postmaterialism thesis posits that
materialism and "traditional values" are negatively correlated with national affluence,
comparisons with national income seem an obvious choice.12 (As we have seen, it posits a
causal relationship, national affluence being an independent, and values dependent variables.)
Inglehart stresses that it is national rather than individual or household affluence that gives
rise to postmaterialism (Inglehart and Abramson 1994). Comparisons between his
Postmaterialism index and per capita GDP are often included in support of the proposition
that from poor to rich countries, there is an increase in the portion of postmaterialists. This
relationship is captured in Figure 6 which shows a strong negative linear relationship between
per capita GDP and the Postmaterialism Index (upper left diagram). Inglehart's index steadily
decreases from the poorest to the richest nations. Using our materialism scores from the
rotated MCA solution, the picture is more complex, suggesting a curvilinear, rather than
linear relationship (upper right diagram in Figure 6). In contrast with the high scores of
Inglehart's Index, our new materialism score is very low among the poorest countries. It then
sharply increases in the 3 to 10 thousand dollars per capita GDP range before dropping again
below zero. But this is not to say that we have identified a mechanism whereby small
increments in overall affluence first give rise to an increase, and, at much higher levels to a
decrease in materialistic aspirations. That is because most societies in the lower-middle per
12
As with the cloud of individuals, these associations need to be treated with caution until the equivalence of constructs is
established. For this reason, regressions are not included in this section.
21
capita GDP range (where our materialism score reaches its highest values) have lived, until
the early 1990s under authoritarian regimes, and nearly all positive materialism scores belong
to these post-authoritarian regimes (marked in different data points, postcommunist countries
for the most part). Their higher materialism scores suggest that materialism has probably less
to do with overall affluence than other contextual variables. In the case of societies under
post-authoritarian regimes, most of their data come from the early phase of their
democratization marked by rapid privatization, rising social inequalities and unemployment,
and the worship of entrepreneurship and material success by the new elites and their official
ideologues. We cannot at this level determine the extent to which these or other factors
contribute to the higher materialism of the lower middle per capita GDP range but can
reasonably suggest that overall affluence is not a major factor.
[Figure 6 about here]
What actually decreases from poor to rich countries is authoritarianism (second diagram
on the right in Figure 6), giving further support to the already corroborated claim that
Inglehart's Postmaterialism Index taps the libertarian-authoritarian cleavage rather than
(post)materialism. Moreover, per capita GDP's negative correlation with authoritarianism is a
great deal stronger than with Inglehart's traditional-secular measure (bottom left diagram)
because the latter is composed of items related to religiosity as well as authoritarianism. From
poor to rich countries, the decrease in religiosity is much less pronounced (bottom right
diagram) than the drop in authoritarianism.
The above comparisons suggest that the relationship between values and
macroeconomic conditions, specifically national affluence is not in line with the
postmaterialism thesis and its offshoots. With the exception of authoritarianism, differences
22
in per capita GDP correspond to only small differences in values. With regard to materialism,
even small differences appear to be related to other variables ― most importantly, the
historical context ― rather than national affluence. Historical context might also explain
away a great deal of the correlation between national affluence and authoritarianism. Were we
to use Inglehart's instruments, these particularities would not come to light. These findings
support a key insight in field analysis which, by virtue of its focus on relations stresses the
stability of social configurations (Martin 2003). Such stability is at odds with the mechanistic
view prevailing in the sociology of variables where variations in some "ultimate" factors
induce profound changes in the social context.
Conclusion
The results presented in this article confirm what many Inglehart critics have long been
suggesting. Rather than being a showcase of "scholarly sophistication", instruments adapted
to the complexities of the individual level are a requirement to better grasp the content of
values and the direction of cultural change. The apparent efficiency of metric methods applied
to aggregate data comes at the price of reductionist typologies and indicators. Specifically,
religiosity, authoritarianism, and materialism constitute distinct value domains that should not
be subsumed under broader constructs.
Overlooking the complexities of individual value configurations has also led to
misconceptions about their "origins" on one hand and "consequences" on the other,
sometimes in the form of contradictory findings even within the same school of research.
Inglehart's work on values is a demonstration of this struggle where the methodological
apparatus follows from misplaced theoretical perspectives. Hence the reorientation on the
23
actor-field dialectic called for in value research also requires that the culture-economy
dichotomy be abandoned as empirically irrelevant.
24
Tables and Figures
Table 1 The 12-item battery of materialist and postmaterialist measures
Materialist Measures
Postmaterialist Measures
A) Maintaining order in the nation*
B) Giving people more say in decisions on
the government*
C) Fighting rising prices*
D) Protecting freedom of speech*
E) Maintaining a high rate of economic
growth
G) Giving people more say in how things are
decided at work and in their community
F) Making sure the country has strong
defense forces
H) Trying to make our cities and countryside
more beautiful
I) Maintaining a stable economy
K) Moving towards a friendlier, less
impersonal society
J) Fighting against crime
L) Moving towards s society where ideas
count more than money
*Items used for the 4-item materialist/postmaterialist battery.
Reproduced from Inglehart 1997: 355. The Postmaterialism Index is calculated using a 4- or 12-item battery pertaining to
national priorities and policy preferences as perceived by the respondent. The 4-item index is constructed as follows: for the
questions on the first and the second most important national priorities, respondents selecting both "maintaining order in the
nation" (A) and "fighting rising prices" (C) are classified as materialists, while those selecting both "giving people more say in
decisions on the government" (B) and "protecting freedom of speech" (D) are classified as postmaterialists. Those selecting both
a "materialist" and a "postmaterialist" item are classified as mixed. This 3-point scale is available for most WVS country
surveys, whereas the 12-item index (on a 6-point scale, constructed from questions A to L in three different sets on policy goals)
is only available for a limited number of countries and waves.
Table 2 The Materialist/Postmaterialist Dimension in 15 Western Nations, 1990 (Loadings on First Principal
Component in Factor Analysis)
Item
K) Less Impersonal Society
G) More Say On Job
B) More Say in Government
L) Ideas Count More than Money
D) Freedom of Speech
H) More Beautiful Cities
Loading
0.60
0.62
0.50
0.51
0.34
0.18
F) Strong Defense Forces
C) Fight Rising Prices
J) Fight against Crime
A) Maintain Order
E) Economic Growth
I) Maintain Stable Economy
-0,26
-0,28
-0,39
-0,55
-0,59
-0,63
Items with materialist polarity are in boldface type; items with Postmaterialist polarity are in italics.
Reproduced from Inglehart 1997: 112, table 4.1 (codes referring to Table 1 added), based on 1990-91 World Values Survey data
from France, Britain, West Germany, Italy, the Netherlands, Denmark, Belgium, Ireland, Norway, Sweden, Denmark, Finland,
Iceland, Canada, and the United States.
25
Figure 1 Inglehart’s two-dimensional cultural typology: “Traditional v. secular/rational” and “Survival v. wellbeing” values
Secular/Rational Authority
Survival
Well-Being
Traditional Authority
Reproduced from Inglehart 1997: 82, Figure 3.2. Data are from the 1990-93 wave of the World Values Survey. The figure
shows the first and the second principal components (horizontal and vertical axes respectively, accounting for 30 and 21% of
the variance) emerging from a factor analysis of data from representative national surveys of 43 societies, aggregated to the
national level. The positioning of the items reflects their loadings on the two dimensions.
26
Table 3 Items Characterizing Two Dimensions of Cross-Cultural Variations
Traditional v. Secular-Rational Valuesa
Survival v. Self-Expression Valuesc
TRADITIONAL VALUES EMPHASIZE
THE FOLLOWING:
SURVIVAL VALUES EMPHASIZE THE
FOLLOWING:
God is very important in respondent’s life
Respondent gives priority to economic and
physical security over self-expression and
quality-of-lifed
It is more important for a child to learn
obedience and religious faith than
independence and determinationb
Respondent describes self as not very happy
Respondent has not signed and would not
sign a petition
Abortion is never justifiable
Respondent has strong sense of national pride
Homosexuality is never justifiable
Respondent favors more respect for authority
You have to be very careful about trusting
people
(SECULAR-RATIONAL VALUES
EMPHASIZE THE OPPOSITE)
(SELF-EXPRESSION VALUES
EMPHASIZE THE OPPOSITE)
Principal components from a factor analysis Inglehart-Baker (2000: 24) without the factor loadings present in the original table.
Nation-level and individual-level data from 65 societies surveyed in the 1990-91 and 1995-98 World Values Surveys. Compared
with the analysis shown in Figure 1, this is a reduced, 10-item battery. The Survival v. Self-Expression scale is identical with
the Survival v. Well-Being scale in Figure 1.
a
Explains 44 percent of cross-national variation and 26 percent of individual variation
b
Autonomy index
c
Explains 26 percent of cross-national variation and 13 percent of individual variation
d
Measured by the four-item materialist/postmaterialist values index.
27
Figure 2 Global Cultural Map: Locations of 65 Societies on Two Dimensions of Cross-Cultural Variation: World
Values Surveys, 1990–1991 and 1995–1998
Reproduced from Inglehart-Baker 2000: 29, Figure 1. The scales on each axis indicate the country’s factor scores on the given
dimension. The positions of Colombia and Pakistan are estimated from incomplete data.
28
Figure 3 Cloud of modalities from unrotated MCA solution in plane 1-2 (upper map) and 3-4 (lower map)
-0.7
-0.2
0.3
Survival (+)
-1.2
0.8
0.8
THRIFT
God not imp.
no comfort
from religion
REJECTS OUTGROUPS
NO SUBVERSIVE ACTION
no life after death
CHILDLESS
WOMAN NOT
0.3
OK
divorce not OK
ACHIEVEMENT
service: never
Axis 2 [λ2=0.064]
FIGHTING RISING
PRICES MOST IMP .
GOOD PAY MOST IMP.
NOT IMP.
single mother
not OK
learn faith not imp.
God imp.
divorce OK
NO THRIFT
comfort from
religion
learn faith imp.
life after death
GOOD PAY NOT IMP.
-0.2
service: 1+/month
SUBVERSIVE ACTION
(-) Self-expression
CHILDLESS WOMAN
OK
RESPECT PARENTS
CONDITIONAL
IMAGINATION
-0.7
ACHIEVEMENT
MOST IMP .
-1.2
(-) Secular
Axis 1 [λ1=0.271]
Traditional (+)
Multiple correspondence analysis of 19 variables from four waves of World Values Survey data, 1981-2004. See Table 5 in the
Appendix for recoding rules and Table 4 for contributions to the axes. Only modalities with a contribution exceeding the
average as defined by dividing 100 by the total number of modalities (100/39 = 2.56%) are shown. Modalities with significant
contributions are in lower case for Axis 1 and 3, SMALL CAPITALS for Axis 2 and 4, and italics for more than one axis. The combined
contributions of these modalities to Axes 1 to 4 are 86, 76, 89, and 83% respectively.
Non-mater. & Achiev. imp.? (+)
-0.8
-0.3
0.2
ACHIEVEMENT
MOST IMP.
0.7
SERVICE: NEVER
SINGLE MOTHER
rejects outgroups
NOT OK
Axis 4 [λ4=0.031]
GOOD PAY NOT IMP .
(-) Mater. & Achiev. not imp.?
0.7
DIVORCE NOT
OK
work/life balance
not imp.
0.2
interesting job
interesting job
not imp.
ACHIEVEMENT
work/life balance
most imp.
NOT IMP.
-0.3
DIVORCE OK
SERVICE: OCCASIONAL
SINGLE MOTHER
OK
FIGHTING RISING
PRICES MOST IMP .
GOOD PAY MOST IMP .
-0.8
(-) Non-mater. & Achiev. not imp.?
Axis 3 [λ3 =0.049]
29
Materialist & Achiev. imp.? (+)
Figure 4 Cloud of modalities from rotated MCA solution in plane 1-2 (upper map) and 3-4 (lower map)
-0.7
-0.2
0.3
Authoritarian (+)
-1.2
0.9
REJECT OUTGROUPS
DIVORCE NOT OK
SINGLE MOTHER
NOT OK
NO SUBVERSIVE ACTION
OBEDIENCE
men better
learn faith imp.
political leaders
abortion not OK
THRIFT
CHILDLESS
WOMAN NOT
Axis 2 [λ2 =0.099]
0.8
service: never
3+ children
denomination
God imp.
God not imp.
no denomination
learn faith not imp.
no comfort
from religion
less respect
for authority
0.4
service: 1+/month
euthanasia
not OK prayer outside service
never think about
meaning of life
no life after death
OK
comfort from
religion
life after death
good & evil
not clear
-0.1
independence
marriage outdated
sexual freedom
euthanasia OK
SUBVERSIVE ACTION
abortion OK
SINGLE MOTHER
OK
CHILDLESS WOMAN
(-) Libertarian
suicide OK
prostitution OK
DIVORCE
-0.6
OK
IMAGINATION
OK
RESPECT PARENTS
CONDITIONAL
-1.1
(-) Secular
Axis 1 [λ1=0.163]
Religious (+)
Multiple correspondence analysis of 19 variables from four waves of World Values Survey data, 1981-2004. See Table 5 in the
Appendix for recoding rules and Table 4 for contribution to the axes. Only modalities with a contribution exceeding the average
as defined by dividing 100 by the total number of modalities (100/39 = 2.56%) are shown. Modalities with significant
contributions are in lower case for Axis 1 (religiosity) and 3 (materialism), SMALL CAPITALS for Axis 2 (authoritarianism) and 4
(achievement), and italics for more than one axis. The combined contributions of these modalities to Axes 1 to 4 are 89, 75, 82,
and 84% respectively. Modalities in small lighter fonts are categories of supplementary variables that do not contribute to the
axes and are plotted as an illustration.
-0.8
-0.3
0.7
1.2
1.3
Achievement imp. (+)
Axis 4 [λ4=0.076]
0.2
ACHIEVEMENT
MOST IMP .
WORK /LIFE BALANCE
NOT IMP.
doing an important
job most imp.
0.8
interesting job
not imp.
good pay not imp.
0.3
job useful for
society not imp.
(-) Achievement not imp.
imagination
job useful for
society imp.
-0.2
interesting job
THRIFT
SERVICE: OCCASIONAL
good pay most imp.
material benefits
most imp.
reject outgroups
WORK /LIFE BALANCE
MOST IMP .
ACHIEVEMENT
NOT IMP.
FIGHTING RISING
PRICES MOST IMP .
-0.7
(-) Non-materialist
Axis 3 [λ3 =0.078]
30
Materialist (+)
Table 4 Contribution of modalities to dimensions 1 to 4 in the unrotated and the rotated MCA solutions
Unrotated solution
dim 1
Variable
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
Modality
dim 2
Label
Traditional
Survival
Rotated solution
dim 3
dim 4
dim 1
dim 2
dim 3
dim 4
Materialism &
Achievement
Nonmaterialism &
Achievement
Religiosity
Authoritarianism
Materialism
Achievement
1
respect parents conditional
3.4
4.3
0.3
0.8
1.1
7.3
1.0
0.3
2
always respect parents
1.1
1.1
0.1
0.2
0.4
2.0
0.2
0.1
1
learn faith not imp.
4.2
0.3
0.0
0.0
4.1
0.9
0.2
0.0
2
learn faith imp.
8.7
0.6
0.0
0.1
8.3
2.1
0.3
0.1
1
no imagination
0.2
1.3
0.5
0.0
0.0
1.0
1.0
0.0
2
imagination
0.8
5.4
1.9
0.0
0.1
3.9
4.2
0.2
1
no obedience
0.9
1.0
0.0
0.1
0.4
1.6
0.2
0.2
2
obedience
1.7
1.8
0.0
0.2
0.6
3.1
0.3
0.4
1
interesting job not imp.
0.0
1.3
19.1
0.1
0.0
0.3
17.4
3.0
2
interesting job
0.1
0.7
12.7
0.1
0.1
0.3
11.2
2.1
1
accept outgroups
0.2
1.0
1.9
0.8
0.0
1.7
1.0
0.9
2
reject outgroups
0.9
3.1
6.4
2.7
0.0
5.8
3.6
2.7
1
childless woman not OK
1.0
4.5
0.2
0.8
0.0
5.1
0.1
1.0
2
childless woman OK
1.0
6.9
0.2
1.3
0.0
7.1
0.1
1.4
1
single mother not OK
2.7
0.4
0.6
5.7
0.9
6.0
1.7
0.1
2
single mother OK
2.4
0.9
0.5
6.7
0.5
7.1
1.4
0.1
1
no thrift
0.0
3.5
0.3
0.0
0.2
1.3
0.2
1.8
2
thrift
0.0
7.0
0.5
0.0
0.4
2.8
0.4
3.3
1
no life after death
5.4
3.9
0.0
0.8
8.1
0.0
0.5
0.1
2
life after death
5.0
3.6
0.0
0.8
7.4
0.0
0.3
0.1
1
God not imp.
11.5
2.0
0.1
2.0
14.3
0.2
0.0
0.1
2
God imp.
5.9
1.0
0.1
1.0
7.3
0.1
0.0
0.0
1
no comfort from religion
11.3
1.9
0.1
1.5
13.8
0.4
0.0
0.0
2
comfort from religion
6.7
1.1
0.1
0.9
8.1
0.2
0.0
0.0
1
divorce not OK
3.7
1.2
0.0
4.4
1.4
7.5
0.1
0.2
2
divorce OK
3.3
1.8
0.0
4.8
1.0
8.1
0.0
0.1
1
achievement not imp.
0.1
3.0
2.4
5.0
0.0
0.0
0.1
10.0
2
achievement most imp.
0.5
7.9
6.1
13.3
0.1
0.2
0.3
26.5
1
work/life balance not imp.
0.3
0.3
21.1
3.7
0.2
0.0
7.8
17.5
2
work/life balance most imp.
0.1
0.2
11.7
2.1
0.0
0.0
4.2
9.7
1
good pay not imp.
0.0
3.6
4.4
7.0
0.0
0.0
12.4
1.7
2
good pay most imp.
0.0
6.2
7.3
12.1
0.0
0.1
21.0
2.9
1
fighting rising prices not most imp.
0.1
0.7
0.0
1.2
0.0
0.0
0.6
1.2
2
fighting rising prices most imp.
0.2
3.5
0.0
4.3
0.1
0.2
2.5
5.0
1
no subversive action
0.9
4.6
0.5
0.9
0.0
5.2
1.3
0.1
2
subversive action
1.2
5.4
0.6
1.2
0.1
6.4
1.5
0.1
1
service: never
6.2
0.8
0.2
6.7
8.9
0.1
0.6
1.1
2
service: occasional
0.7
0.5
0.0
6.5
0.3
1.5
1.4
3.3
3
service: 1+/month
7.6
1.8
0.1
0.2
8.2
1.0
0.3
0.7
Contribution exceeding the average as defined by dividing 100 by the total number of modalities (100/39 = 2.56%) are
highlighted in bold.
31
Figure 5 Cloud of individuals from four societies in plane 1-2 (upper map) and 3-4 (lower map) with landmarks
-2.5
-1.5
-0.5
0.5
1.5
2.5
Authoritarian (+)
2.5
122 955
239 119
1.5
Axis 2 [y]
0.5
-0.5
(-) Libertarian
189 046
-1.5
Russia '95
Jordan '01
Colombia '98
Sweden '99
104 551
-2.5
(-) Secular
Axis 1 [x]
Religious (+)
World Values Survey, Russia (1995, N=1.000), Colombia (1998, N=1.000), Sweden (1999, N=1.013) and Jordan (2001, N=1.000).
Data points represent individuals’ position along the four rotated axes of cultural values revealed by multiple correspondence
analysis. The original samples for Russia, Colombia and Jordan include 2.038, 2.996 and 1.223 cases respectively: for ease of
presentation, 1.000 cases were randomly selected preserving the regional breakdown of these original samples. Highlighted
data points represent landmark individuals (marked using their unified respondent number in the WVS) whose description
helps illustrate the four dimensions.
-2.5
-1.5
-0.5
0.5
1.5
2.5
(-) Achievement not imp.
Axis 4 [y]
Achievement imp. (+)
3.0
104 551
2.0
1.0
189 046
0.0
-1.0
Russia '95
Jordan '01
Colombia '98
Sweden '99
122 955
239 119
-2.0
(-) Non-materialist
Axis 3 [x]
32
Materialist (+)
Figure 6 Per capita GDP v. Inglehart's instruments and scores from MCA
Materialism score from rotated MCA solution
1.5
Materialism (Axis 4, rotated MCA solution)
Inglehart's Postmaterialism Index*
Inglehart's Postmaterialism Index (standardized reverse scale)
1.0
0.5
0.0
-0.5
R² = 0.4611
-1.0
-1.5
0
10 000
20 000
30 000
40 000
1.5
R² = 0.1933
1.0
0.5
0.0
-0.5
R² = 0.1016
-1.0
-1.5
0
10 000
2005 Real GDP/capita (PPP)
20 000
30 000
40 000
2005 Real GDP/capita (PPP)
Last available data from the World Values Survey and per capita GDP figures (2005 US dollars at purchasing power parity) from
the Penn World Table (Heston, Summers, and Aten 2002). The number of countries is 76 for the left and 81 for the right
diagrams (excluding Luxembourg as influential case because of its high per capita GDP). Countries marked with round white
data points have recently democratized ("post-authoritarian") governments.
Authoritarianism
Authoritarianism score from rotated MCA solution
1.5
1.0
0.5
0.0
1.5
-1.0
1.0
-1.5
R² = 0.4808
0
10 000
0.5
20 000
30 000
40 000
2005 Real GDP/capita (PPP)
0.0
Religiosity score from rotated MCA solution
-0.5
-1.0
Religiosity
Traditional (+) v. Secular/Rational (-) values
Inglehart's Traditional-secular score
-0.5
R² = 0.1774
-1.5
0
10 000
20 000
30 000
1.5
1.0
0.5
40 000
2005 Real GDP/capita (PPP)
0.0
-0.5
R² = 0.1526
-1.0
-1.5
0
10 000
20 000
30 000
40 000
2005 Real GDP/capita (PPP)
33
Appendix
Table 5 Recoding of questions from the WVS for multiple correspondence analysis
WVS
name
label
(for composite scores, original variables are in italic)
original scale
in WVS
a025
With which of these two statements do you tend to
agree?
A) One most always love and respect parents
(regardless of their qualities and faults).
B) Love and respect must be earned.
1: agree with A
2: agree with B
3: neither
a040
Qualities that children
can be encouraged to
learn at home.
Which, if any, do you
consider to be especially
important?
a129
Which (if any) of these
people would you not
like to have as
neighbors?
a132
c011
always respect
parents
(2)
respect parents
conditional
(1)
imagination
(0, DK)
imagination not
imp.
(1)
thrift
(0, DK)
no thrift
(1)
learn faith imp.
(0, DK)
learn faith not imp.
(1)
obedience
(0, DK)
no obedience
(0)
accepts outgroups
(1, 2, 3)
rejects outgroups
(1)
good pay imp.
(0)
good pay not imp.
(0)
work/life balance
not imp.
(1, 2, 3)
work/life balance
imp.
(0)
achievement not
imp.
(1 thru 4)
achievement imp.
Thrift, saving money and
things
1: important
0: not mentioned
Religious faith
Immigrants/foreign
workers
People who have AIDS
0 to 3, depending on the number of
groups rejected
Homosexuals
1: mentioned
0: not mentioned
Important in a job: good pay
c012
c015
(1)
Obedience
a042
a130
label for modalities
in MCA
Imagination
a034
a038
recoding
rule
(DK=Don't
know)
Not too much pressure
Important in a job:
Good hours
c017
Generous holidays
c014
Respected job
c016
Opportunity to use
initiative
Important in a job:
c018
You can achieve
something
c019
Responsible job
0 to 3, depending on the number of
items mentioned
0 to 4, depending on the number of
items mentioned
34
WVS
name
label
(for composite scores, original variables are in italic)
c020
Important in a job: a job that's interesting?
d019
d023
e003
e026
e028
e029
f028
f051
f063
f064
f121
recoding
rule
(DK=Don't
know)
label for modalities
in MCA
(1)
interesting job
(0)
interesting job. not
imp.
(1)
childless woman
not OK
(2)
childless woman OK
0: disapprove
1: approve
2: depends
(0)
single mother not
OK
(1)
single mother OK
1: maintaining order in the nation
2: give people more say
3: fighting rising prices
4: protecting freedom of speech
(3)
fight rising prices
most imp.
(1, 2, 3,
DK)
fight rising prices
not most imp.
(if 3 or 2
for any)
subversive action
(if no 3 or
2 for any:
0, DK)
no subversive
action
(1, 2, 3)
service: 1+/month
(4, 5, 6, 7)
service: occasional
(8)
service: never
(1)
life after death
(0, DK)
no life after death
(1 thru 5,
DK)
God not imp.
(6 thru 10)
God imp.
(1)
comfort from
religion
(0, DK)
no comfort from
religion
not OK
(1 thru 4)
divorce not OK
OK
(6 thru 10)
divorce OK
original scale
in WVS
1: mentioned
0: not mentioned
1: needs children
2: not necessary
Woman has to have children in order to be fulfilled?
Woman wants to have child as a single parent but
doesn't want to have a stable relationship with a man
If you had to choose, which one of the things would
you say is most important?
Of different forms of
political action, whether
you have actually done,
might do or
would never do any of
these things:
Joining in boycotts
1: have done
2: might do
3: would never do
Joining unofficial strikes
Occupying buildings or
factories
1: 2+/week
2: weekly
3: monthly
4: special holydays
5: other specific holy days
6: once a year
7: less often
8: never, practically never
How often do you attend religious services?
1: yes
0: no
Do you believe in live after death?
How important is God in your life?
1 (not at all) to 10 (very)
1: yes
0: no
Do you get comfort and strength from religion?
1 (never justifiable) to
10 (always justifiable)
Divorce
All variables are recoded into binary modalities except f028 (attendance of religious services), recoded into three categories.
"Don't know" answers are used for a number of questions, as specified in the recoding rules. Variables c011 to c019 (work
attitudes) are combined into new variables based on results from a separate multiple correspondence analysis revealing a twodimensional latent structure.
35
References
Adachi, Kohei. 2004. “Oblique Promax rotation applied to the solutions in multiple
correspondence analysis.” Behaviormetrika 31:1–12.
Bennett, Tony, Mauricio Bustamante, and John Frow. 2013. “The Australian space of
lifestyles in comparative perspective.” Journal of Sociology 49:224–255.
Blasius, Jörg, and Peter Graeff. 2009. “Economic freedom, wealth and corruption: Analyzing
their empirical connection by means of correspondence analysis.” Pp. 135–155 in
Graeff, Peter, and Mehlkop, Guido: Capitalism, democracy and the prevention of war
and poverty.
Blasius, Jörg, and Michael Greenacre. 2006. “Correspondence Analysis and Related Methods
in Practice.” Pp. 3–40 in Blasius, Jörg, and Greenacre, Michael: Multiple
Correspondence Analysis and Related Methods. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman &
Hall/CRC.
Blasius, Jörg, and Andreas Mühlichen. 2010. “Identifying audience segments applying the
‘social space’ approach.” Poetics 38:69–89.
Blasius, Jörg, and Victor Thiessen. 2006a. “A three-step approach to assessing the behaviour
of survey items in cross-national research.” Pp. 433–453 in Blasius, Jörg, and
Greenacre, Michael: Multiple Correspondence Analysis and Related Methods. Boca
Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.
Blasius, Jörg, and Victor Thiessen. 2006b. “Assessing Data Quality and Construct
Comparabilityin Cross-National Surveys.” European Sociological Review 22:229–242.
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1987. Choses dites. Paris: Les Éditions de Minuit.
36
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1979. La distinction. Critique sociale du jugement. Paris: Les Éditions de
Minuit.
Bourdieu, Pierre. 2000. Les structures sociales de l’économie. Paris: Seuil.
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1999. “Une révolution conservatrice dans l’édition.” Actes de la recherche
en sciences sociales 126-127:3–28.
Bourdieu, Pierre, and Monique De Saint Martin. 1978. “Le patronat.” Actes de la recherche
en sciences sociales 20-21:3–82.
Böltken, Ferdinand, and Wolfgang Jagodzinski. 1985. “In an Environment of Insecurity:
Postmaterialism in the European Community, 1970 to 1980.” Comparative Political
Studies 17:453–484.
Buhlmann, Felix, Thomas David, and André Mach. 2013. “Cosmopolitan Capital and the
Internationalization of the Field of Business Elites: Evidence from the Swiss Case.”
Cultural Sociology 7:211–229.
Chalcraft, David J. 2005. “Reading Weber’s Patterns of Response to Critics of The Protestant
Ethic: Some ‘Affinities’ in and between Replies to Felix Rachfahl and Werner
Sombart.” Journal of Classical Sociology 5:31–51.
Chalcraft, David J., and Austin (eds.) Harrington. 2001. The Protestant ethic debate: Max
Weber’s replies to his critics, 1907-1910. Liverpool: Liverpool University Press.
Clarke, Harold D., and Nitish Dutt. 1991. “Measuring Value Change in Western
Industrialized Societies: The Impact of Unemployment.” The American Political Science
Review 85:905.
Clarke, Harold D., Nitish Dutt, and Jonathan Rapkin. 1997a. “(Mis)Measuring Value Change:
A Brief Postscript.” Political Behavior 19:61–64.
37
Clarke, Harold D., Nitish Dutt, and Jonathan Rapkin. 1997b. “Conversations in context: The
(mis) measurement of value change in advanced industrial societies.” Political Behavior
19:19–40.
Davidov, Eldad, Peter Schmidt, and Shalom H. Schwartz. 2008. “Bringing Values Back In:
The Adequacy of the European Social Survey to Measure Values in 20 Countries.”
Public Opinion Quarterly 72:420–445.
Davis, Darren W., and Christian Davenport. 1999. “Assessing the Validity of the
Postmaterialism Index.” Political Science 93:649–664.
Davis, Darren W., Kathleen M. Dowley, and Brian D. Silver. 1999. “Postmaterialism in
World Societies: Is It Really a Value Dimension?” American Journal of Political
Science 43:935 – 962.
Deth, Jan W. Van. 1983. “The Persistence of Materialist and Post-Materialist Value
Orientations.” European Journal of Political Research 11:63–79.
Duch, Raymond M., and Michaell A. Taylor. 1994. “A Reply to Abramson and Inglehart’s
‘Education, Security, and Postmaterialism.’” American Journal of Political Science
38:815.
Duch, Raymond M., and Michaell A. Taylor. 1993. “Postmaterialism and the Economic
Condition.” American Journal of Political Science 37:747.
Flanagan, Scott C. 1982a. “Changing Values in Advanced Industrial Societies: Inglehart’s
Silent Revolution from the Perspective of Japanese Findings.” Comparative Political
Studies 14:403–444.
Flanagan, Scott C. 1982b. “Measuring Value Change in Advanced Industrial Societies: A
Rejoinder to Inglehart.” Comparative Political Studies 15:99–128.
Flemmen, Magne. 2012. “The Structure of the Upper Class: A Social Space Approach.”
Sociology 46:1039–1058.
38
Graaf, Nan Dirk De, and Geoffrey Evans. 1996. “Why are the Young more Postmaterialist?:
A Cross-National Analysis of Individual and Contextual Influences on Postmaterial
Values.” Comparative Political Studies 28:608–635.
Granato, Jim, Ronald Inglehart, and David Leblang. 1996a. “Cultural Values, Stable
Democracy, and Economic Development: A Reply.” American Journal of Political
Science 40:680–696.
Granato, Jim, Ronald Inglehart, and David Leblang. 1996b. “The Effect of Cultural Values on
Economic Development: Theory, Hypotheses, and Some Empirical Tests.” American
Journal of Political Science 40:607–631.
Haller, Max. 2002. “Theory and Method in the Comparative Study of Values: Critique and
Alternative to Inglehart.” European Sociological Review 18:139–158.
Hanquinet, Laurie. 2013. “Visitors to modern and contemporary art museums: towards a new
sociology of ‘cultural profiles.’” The Sociological Review 61:790–813.
Hanquinet, Laurie, Henk Roose, and Mike Savage. 2013. “The Eyes of the Beholder:
Aesthetic Preferences and the Remaking of Cultural Capital.” Sociology 48:111–132.
Harrits, Gitte Sommer, Annick Prieur, Lennart Rosenlund, and Jakob Skjott-Larsen. 2010.
“Class and Politics in Denmark: Are Both Old and New Politics Structured by Class?”
Scandinavian Political Studies 33:1–27.
Hendricks, J., and C. Breckinridge Peters. 1973. “The Ideal Type and Sociological Theory.”
Acta Sociologica 16:31–40.
Heston, Alan, Robert Summers, and Bettina Aten. 2002. “Penn World Table.” Center for
International Comparisons at the University of Pennsylvania.
Hjellbrekke, Johs. et al. 2007. “The Norwegian Field of Power Anno 2000.” European
Societies 9:245–273.
39
Howe, Richard Herbert. 1978. “Max Weber’s Elective Affinities: Sociology Within the
Bounds of Pure Reason.” The American Journal of Sociology 84:366–385.
Inglehart, Ronald. 1997. Modernization and Postmodernization. Cultural, Economic and
Political Change in 43 Societies. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Inglehart, Ronald. 1983. “The Persistence of Materialist and Post-Materialist Value
Orientations: Comments on Van Deth’s Analysis.” European Journal of Political
Research 11:81–91.
Inglehart, Ronald. 1971. “The Silent Revolution in Europe: Intergenerational Change in PostIndustrial Societies.” The American Political Science Review 65:991–1017.
Inglehart, Ronald. 1977. The Silent Revolution: Changing Values and Political Styles among
Western Publics. Princeton University Press.
Inglehart, Ronald, and Paul R. Abramson. 1994. “Economic Security and Value Change.”
The American Political Science Review 88:336–354.
Inglehart, Ronald, and Wayne E. Baker. 2000. “Modernization, cultural change, and the
persistence of traditional values.” American Sociological Review 65:19–51.
Inglehart, Ronald, and Scott C. Flanagan. 1987. “Value change in industrial societies.” The
American Political Science Review 81:1289–1319.
Kaiser, Henry F. 1958. “The varimax criterion for analytic rotation in factor analysis.”
Psychometrika 23:187–200.
Kiers, Henk A. L. 1991. “Simple structure in component analysis techniques for mixtures of
qualitative and quantitative variables.” Psychometrika 56:197–212.
Kropp, Kristoffer. 2013. “Social sciences in the field of power - the case of Danish social
science.” Social Science Information 52:425–449.
40
Lafferty, William M., and Oddbjørn Knutsen. 1985. “Postmaterialism in a Social Democratic
State: An Analysis of the Distinctness and Congruity of the Inglehart Value Syndrome in
Norway.” Comparative Political Studies 17:411–430.
Lebaron, Frédéric, and Brigitte Le Roux. 2011. “Social capital in the field of power : the case
of Norway.” Review Literature And Arts Of The Americas 1:86–108.
Lorenzo-Seva, Urbano, Michel van de Velden, and Henk A. L. Kiers. 2009. “CAR: A
MATLAB Package to Compute Correspondence Analysis with Rotations.” Journal of
Statistical Software 31:1–14.
Löwy, Michael. 2004. “Le concept d’affinité élective chez Max Weber.” Archives de sciences
sociales des religions 93–103.
Lucas, Robert E. 1988. “On The Mechanics of Economic Development.” Journal of
Monetary Economics 22:3–42.
Majima, Shinolu, and Mike Savage. 2007. “Have There Been Culture Shifts in Britain?: A
Critical Encounter with Ronald Inglehart.” Cultural Sociology 1:293–315.
Marsh, Alan. 1975. “The ‘Silent Revolution,’ Value Priorities, and the Quality of Life in
Britain.” The American Political Science Review 69:21–30.
Martin, John Levi. 2003. “What Is Field Theory?” American Journal of Sociology 109:1–49.
Maslow, Abraham Harold. 1970. Motivation and Personality. New York: Harper & Row.
McKinnon, Andrew M. 2010. “Elective Affinities of the Protestant Ethic: Weber and the
Chemistry of Capitalism.” Sociological Theory 28:108–126.
Mills, Charles Wright. 1940. “Situated Actions and Vocabularies of Motive.” American
Sociological Review 5:904–913.
Purhonen, Semi, and David Wright. 2013. “Methodological Issues in National-Comparative
Research on Cultural Tastes: The Case of Cultural Capital in the UK and Finland.”
Cultural Sociology 7:257–273.
41
Romer, Paul M. 1990. “Endogenous Technological Change.” The Journal of Political
Economy 98:S71–S102.
Rouanet, Henry, Wemer Ackermann, and Brigitte Le Roux. 2000. “The Geometric Analysis
of Questionnaires: the Lesson of Bourdieu’s La Distinction.” Bulletin de Méthodologie
Sociologique 65:5–18.
Roux, Brigitte Le, Henry Rouanet, Mike Savage, and Alan Warde. 2008. “Class and Cultural
Division in the UK.” Sociology 42:1049–1071.
Runciman, W. G. 2005. “Not Elective but Selective Affinities.” Journal of Classical
Sociology 5:175–187.
Saint Martin, Monique De, and Pierre Bourdieu. 1987. “Agrégation et ségrégation [Le champ
des grandes écoles et le champ du pouvoir].” Actes de la recherche en sciences sociales
69:2–50.
Saracco, Jérôme, Marie Chavent, and Vanessa Kuentz. 2010. “Rotation in Multiple
Correspondence Analysis: a planar rotation iterative procedure.” Working Papers pf
GREThA 04.
Savage, Mike, and Elizabeth B. Silva. 2013. “Field Analysis in Cultural Sociology.” Cultural
Sociology 7:111–126.
Schwartz, Shalom H. 2011. “Studying Values: Personal Adventure, Future Directions.”
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 42:307–319.
Silva, Elizabeth B., and Brigitte Le Roux. 2011. “Cultural capital of couples: Tensions of
elective affinities.” Poetics 39:547–565.
Silva, Elizabeth B., and David Wright. 2009. “Displaying Desire and Distinction in Housing.”
Cultural Sociology 3:31–50.
Solow, Robert M. 1956. “A Contribution to the Theory of Economic Growth.” The Quarterly
Journal of Economics 70:65–94.
42
Swan, Trevor W. 1956. “Economic Growth and Capital Accumulation.” Economic Record
32:334–361.
Treviño, A. Javier. 2005. “Parsons’s Action-System Requisite Model and Weber's Elective
Affinity: A Convergence of Convenience.” Journal of Classical Sociology 5:319–348.
Trump, Thomas M. 1991. “Value Formation and Postmaterialism: Inglehart’s Theory of
Value Change Reconsidered.” Comparative Political Studies 24:365–390.
Veenstra, Gerry. 2007. “Social space, social class and Bourdieu: health inequalities in British
Columbia, Canada.” Health & place 13:14–31.
Velden, Michel, and Henk A. L. Kiers. 2005. “Rotation in Correspondence Analysis.”
Journal of Classification 22:251–271.
Vijver, F. J. R. van De, and Ype H. Poortinga. 2002. “Structural Equivalence in Multilevel
Research.” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 33:141–156.
Wacquant, Loïc. 2013. “Symbolic power and group-making: On Pierre Bourdieu’s reframing
of class.” Journal of Classical Sociology 13:274–291.
Weber, Max, Peter R. Baehr, and Gordon C. Wells. 2002. The Protestant Ethic and the
“Spirit” of Capitalism and Other Writings. New York: Penguin Classics.
Welzel, Christian. 2002. “Effective Democracy, Mass Culture, and the Quality of Elites: The
Human Development Perspective.” International Journal of Comparative Sociology
43:317–349.
Welzel, Christian, Ronald Inglehart, and Hans-Dieter Kligemann. 2003. “The theory of
human development: A cross-cultural analysis.” European Journal of Political Research
42:341–379.
World Values Survey Association. 2009. “1981-2008 Official Aggregate v.20090901
Aggregate File Producer: ASEP/JDS, Madrid (www.worldvaluessurvey.org).”
43