Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Number Sense Series Tips

Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Number Sense Series: Developing

Early Number Sense


Stage: 1
Article by Jenni Way

You can read an updated version of this article in our Number Sense and
Place Value Feature.
What is number sense?

The term "number sense" is a relatively new one in mathematics education.
It is difficult to define precisely, but broadly speaking, it refers to "a well
organised conceptual framework of number information that enables a
person to understand numbers and number relationships and to solve
mathematical problems that are not bound by traditional algorithms" (Bobis,
1996). The National Council of Teachers (USA, 1989) identified five
components that characterise number sense: number meaning, number
relationships, number magnitude, operations involving numbers and
referents for numbers and quantities. These skills are considered important
because they contribute to general intuitions about numbers and lay the
foundation for more advanced skills.

Researchers have linked good number sense with skills observed in students
proficient in the following mathematical activities:

mental calculation (Hope & Sherrill, 1987;
Trafton, 1992);
computational estimation (for example;
Bobis, 1991; Case & Sowder, 1990);
judging the relative magnitude of numbers
(Sowder, 1988);
recognising part-whole relationships and
place value concepts (Fischer, 1990; Ross,
1989) and;
problem solving (Cobb et.al., 1991).



How does number sense begin?
An intuitive sense of number begins at a very early age. Children as young
as two years of age can confidently identify one, two or three objects before
they can actually count with understanding (Gelman & Gellistel, 1978).
Piaget called this ability to instantaneously recognise the number of objects
in a small group 'subitising'. As mental powers develop, usually by about the
age of four, groups of four can be recognised without counting. It is thought
that the maximum number for subitising, even for most adults, is five. This
skill appears to be based on the mind's ability to form stable mental images
of patterns and associate them with a number. Therefore, it may be possible
to recognise more than five objects if they are arranged in a particular way
or practice and memorisation takes place. A simple example of this is six
dots arranged in two rows of three, as on dice or playing cards. Because this
image is familiar, six can be instantly recognised when presented this way.
Usually, when presented with more than five objects, other mental
strategies must be utilised. For example, we might see a group of six objects
as two groups of three. Each group of three is instantly recognised, then
very quickly (virtually unconsciously) combined to make six. In this strategy
no actual counting of objects is involved, but rather a part-part-whole
relationship and rapid mental addition is used. That is, there is an
understanding that a number (in this case six) can be composed of smaller
parts, together with the knowledge that 'three plus three makes six'. This
type of mathematical thinking has already begun by the time children begin
school and should be nurtured because it lays the foundation for
understanding operations and in developing valuable mental calculation
strategies.

What teaching strategies promote early number sense?

Learning to count with understanding is a crucial number skill, but other
skills, such as perceiving subgroups, need to develop alongside counting to
provide a firm foundation for number sense. By simply presenting objects
(such as stamps on a flashcard) in various arrangements, different mental
strategies can be prompted. For example, showing six stamps in a cluster of
four and a pair prompts the combination of 'four and two makes six'. If the
four is not subitised, it may be seen as 'two and two and two makes six'.
This arrangement is obviously a little more complex than two groups of
three. So different arrangements will prompt different strategies, and these
strategies will vary from person to person.

If mental strategies such as these are to be encouraged (and just counting
discouraged) then an element of speed is necessary. Seeing the objects for
only a few seconds challenges the mind to find strategies other than
counting. It is also important to have children reflect on and share their
strategies (Presmeg, 1986; Mason, 1992). This is helpful in three ways:

verbalising a strategy brings the strategy to
a conscious level and allows the person to
learn about their own thinking;
it provides other children with the
opportunity to pick up new strategies;
the teacher can assess the type of thinking
being used and adjust the type of
arrangement, level of difficulty or speed of
presentation accordingly.

To begin with, early number activities are best done with moveable objects
such as counters, blocks and small toys. Most children will need the concrete
experience of physically manipulating groups of objects into sub-groups and
combining small groups to make a larger group. After these essential
experiences more static materials such as 'dot cards' become very useful.

Dot cards are simply cards with dot stickers of a single colour stuck on one
side. (However, any markings can be used. Self-inking stamps are fast when
making a lot of cards). The important factors in the design of the cards are
the number of dots and the arrangement of these dots. The various
combinations of these factors determine the mathematical structure of each
card, and hence the types of number relations and mental strategies
prompted by them.

Consider each of the following arrangements of dots before reading further.
What mental strategies are likely to be prompted by each card? What order
would you place them in according to level of difficulty?


Card A is the classic symmetrical dice and playing card arrangement of five
and so is often instantly recognised without engaging other mental
strategies. It is perhaps the easiest arrangement of five to deal with.

Card B presents clear sub-groups of two and three, each of which can be
instantly recognised. With practice, the number fact of 'two and three makes
five' can be recalled almost instantly.

Card C: A linear arrangement is the one most likely to prompt counting.
However, many people will mentally separate the dots into groups of two
and three, as in the previous card. Other strategies such as seeing two then
counting '3, 4, 5' might also be used.

Card D could be called a random arrangement, though in reality it has been
quite deliberately organised to prompt the mental activity of sub-grouping.
There are a variety of ways to form the sub-groups, with no prompt in any
particular direction, so this card could be considered to be the most difficult
one in the set.

Card E shows another sub-group arrangement that encourages the use (or
discovery) of the 'four and one makes five' number relation.

Obviously, using fewer than five dots would develop the most basic number
sense skills, and using more than five dots would provide opportunities for
more advanced strategies. However, it is probably not useful to use more
than ten dots. (See the follow-on article focusing on developing a 'sense of
ten' and 'place value readiness'). Cards such as these can be shown briefly
to children, then the children asked how many dots they saw. The children
should be asked to explain how they perceived the arrangement, and hence
what strategies they employed.

What games can assist development of early number sense?

Games can be very useful for reinforcing and developing ideas and
procedures previously introduced to children. Although a suggested age
group is given for each of the following games, it is the children's level of
experience that should determine the suitability of the game. Several
demonstration games should be played, until the children become
comfortable with the rules and procedures of the games.

Deal and Copy (4-5 years) 3-4 players

Materials: 15 dot cards with a variety of dot patterns representing the
numbers from one to five and a plentiful supply of counters or buttons.

Rules: One child deals out one card face up to each other player. Each child
then uses the counters to replicate the arrangement of dots on his/her card
and says the number aloud. The dealer checks each result, then deals out a
new card to each player, placing it on top of the previous card. The children
then rearrange their counters to match the new card. This continues until all
the cards have been used.

Variations/Extensions
1. Each child can predict aloud whether the
new card has more, less or the same
number of dots as the previous card. The
prediction is checked by the dealer, by
observing whether counters need to be
taken away or added.
2. Increase the number of dots on the cards.
Memory Match (5-7 years) 2 players
Materials: 12 dot cards, consisting of six pairs of cards showing two different
arrangements of a particular number of dots, from 1 to 6 dots. (For
example, a pair for 5 might be Card A and Card B from the set above).
Rules: Spread all the cards out face down. The first player turns over any
two cards. If they are a pair (i.e. have the same number of dots), the player
removes the cards and scores a point. If they are not a pair, both cards are
turned back down in their places. The second player then turns over two
cards and so on. When all the cards have been matched, the player with
more pairs wins.
Variations/Extensions
1. Increase the number of pairs of cards used.
2. Use a greater number of dots on the cards.
3. Pair a dot card with a numeral card.
What's the Difference? (7-8 years) 2-4 players
Materials: A pack of 20 to 30 dot cards (1 to 10 dots in dice and regular
patterns), counters.
Rules: Spread out 10 cards face down and place the rest of the cards in a
pile face down. The first player turns over the top pile card and places beside
the pile. He/she then turns over one of the spread cards. The player works
out the difference between the number of dots on each card, and takes that
number of counters. (E.g. If one card showed 3 dots and the other 8, the
player would take 5 counters.) The spread card is turned face down again in
its place and the next player turns the top pile card and so on. Play
continues until all the pile cards have been used. The winner is the player
with the most counters; therefore the strategy is to remember the value of
the spread cards so the one that gives the maximum difference can be
chosen.
Variations/Extensions
1. Try to turn the spread cards that give the
minimum difference, so the winner is the
player with the fewest counters.
2. Roll a die instead of using pile cards. Start
with a set number of counters (say 20), so
that when all the counters have been
claimed the game ends.
3. Use dot cards with random arrangements of
dots.
The next article in this series is entitled A Sense of 'ten' and Place Value.

References
Bobis, J. (1991). The effect of instruction on the development of
computation estimation strategies. Mathematics Education Research Journal
, 3, 7-29.

Bobis, J. (1996). Visualisation and the development of number sense with
kindergarten children. In Mulligan, J. & Mitchelmore, M. (Eds.) Children's
Number Learning : A Research Monograph of the Mathematics Education
Group of Australasia and the Australian Association of Mathematics
Teachers. Adelaide: AAMT

Case, R. & Sowder, J. (1990). The development of computational estimation:
A neo-Piagetian analysis. Cognition and Instruction , 7, 79-104.

Cobb, P., Wood, T., Yackel, E., Nicholls, J., Wheatley, G., Trigatti, B., &
Perlwitz, M., (1991). Assessment of a problem-centred second-grade
mathematics project. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education , 22, 3-
29.

Fischer, F. (1990). A part-part-whole curriculum for teaching number to
kindergarten. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education , 21, 207-215.

Gelman, R. & Gallistel, C. (1978). The Child's Understanding of Number.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Hope, J. & Sherril, J. (1987). Characteristics of unskilled and skilled mental
calculators. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education , 18, 98-111.

Mason, J. (1992). Doing and construing mathematics in screen space, In
Perry, B., Southwell, B., & Owens, K. (Eds.). Proceedings of the Thirteenth
Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of
Australasia . Nepean, Sydney: MERGA.

Ross, S. (1989). Parts, wholes, and place value: A developmental view.
Arithmetic Teacher , 36, 47-51.

Sowder, J. (1988). Mental computation and number comparison: Their roles
in the development of number sense and computational estimation. In
Heibert & Behr (Eds.). Research Agenda for Mathematics Education: Number
Concepts and Operations in the Middle Grades (pp. 192-197). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence, Erlbaum & Reston.

Presmeg, N. (1986). Visualisation in high school mathematics. For the
Learning of Mathematics , 6 (3), 42-46.

Trafton, P. (1992). Using number sense to develop mental computation and
computational estimation. In C. Irons (Ed.) Challenging Children to Think
when they Compute . (pp. 78-92). Brisbane: Centre for Mathematics and
Science Education, Queensland University of Technology



There's no doubt about it...subitizing is all the rage! It's not a new
concept, though. According to Wikipedia the phrase was first used in
1949. So why are we just hearing about it now? My theory is that the
popularity of subitizing is directly related to the implementation of the
Common Core State Standards for Mathematics, which stress conceptual
understanding and number sense over rote memorization
of mathematical procedures and facts.

Not surprisingly, John Van de Walle's books have featured activities
related to subitizing for many years. I enlarged some of Van de Walle's
cards (shown below) so they would be easier to use, and you can grab
them here.

You can also easily make dot cards using large index cards and colored
labels. Van de Walle suggests using paper plates, but I find index cards
or cardstock cut in half are easier to store and use. I hole punch them
and put them on an O-ring. Make multiple configurations of each
number. In the picture below, you see four different cards showing the
number 3. Using different colors of dots helps students see how smaller
numbers combine to make larger ones.



So, now that you've made your dot cards, how exactly do you use
them? First off, you want to develop routines for using your dot
cards. You don't want to flash a card and just have students shout out
the number. Start out, obviously, with smaller numbers. Even if you are
subitizing in 1st or 2nd grade, if your kiddos haven't subitized before,
you need to start with smaller numbers. I like to have kiddos seated on
the floor in front of me. I use a strategy I saw on the DVD that
accompanies Sherry Parrish's book, Number Talks: Helping Children
Build Mental Math and Computation Strategies. Flash a card
briefly. When a child thinks they know the number shown on the card,
they put a thumbs-up against their chest. Totally silent! You want to be
able to look for those kiddos who are counting the dots one-by-one and
also see how quickly each student determines the number. Call on
"thumbs" one by one and ask what number they saw. Don't be surprised
if you get different responses! Avoid the temptation to comment on
responses. Use a good poker face and just accept all answers. Next,
show the card again and call on several students to tell you how they
knew what number was on the card. There is no "right"
answer. Looking at the cards above, you might have kids say:
I saw 2 and 1 more and 2 plus 1 equals 3
I just saw 3
I counted...1, 2, 3
There's 1 blue dot and 2 red dots and that makes 3

Notice how each of those responses tells you something very important
about the child's understanding of numbers. Repeat for different
cards. This is a quick, whole-class daily routine that has a huge
payout! You also want to use the dot cards, however, in small group
instruction to differentiate your instruction. For example, the first kiddo
in the example above is probably ready to work with larger numbers and
combinations while the third kiddo is not subitizing at all.

5- and 10-frames are powerful tools for building those all-important
benchmarks of 5 and 10, and they should also be part of your subitizing
routines. They can be used in much the same way as random-pattern dot
cards. A great resource for using 10-frames in your classroom is It
Makes Sense!: Using Ten-Frames to Build Number Sense. Click here to
download blank ten-frames (3 to a page) to make your dot cards.



Go forth and subitize!!

You might also want to check out these posts on DIY rekenreks and
number bracelets.

You might also like