Learning in Neural Models With Complex Dynamics : Michael Stiber Segundo
Learning in Neural Models With Complex Dynamics : Michael Stiber Segundo
405
s2
I
Presynaptic Rate
‘5
to tlt2
406
1x10-6
-.->,
c
B
!ti 1x10.~
n
.-
w
0 1 1 1
8. fi f2 f3
f.
cn *
f, inputfrequency
E
‘8 1x10-8 Fig. 4. Illustration of effects of changing synaptic strength, w ,
on neural behavior. The shaded area represents part of a locking
A tongue in an Amol’d map. A small change in synaptic weight
3Y
from w1 to w2 can change a non-locked behavior at f3 to a
locked one, or have no effect at all, such as at fi or fi.
Ini
+ +
between p : q and p’ :q‘ being p p’ : q q’ (not readily ap-
1 parent in the figure because of the coarse sampling of the pa-
1~10-~ I rameters). Between these tongues lie parameter regions which
I I I I I I I I
produced nonlocked behaviors (cross-hatched areas), including
0.2 0.4 Q 6 0.8 1.2 1.4 1.6 1.8
all of those seen in the Iiv5g preparation. The “horizontal slice”
NA, Normalized Input Frequency at any paaicular value of P , corresponds to a one-dimensional
bifurcation diagram; if we were to plot average postsynaptic rate
Fig. 3. Amol’d map from simulations with inhibitory input. instead of behavioral category, we would produce a graph similar
White areas indicate parameter ranges which produced lockings to Fig. 2.
(ratios noted). Cross-hatched areas are non-locked behaviors. How does changing the synaptic strength change the behavior
pattem? The locking tongues extend all the way down to values
of Fsy,,that no longer have significant practical effects on behav-
monotonic, and those monotonic areas’ slopes are opposite the ior. However, they narrow as pJy,, decreases. This corresponds
overall trend. In contrast, the output of a typical ANN model is to reducing the widths of the paradoxically-slopedline segments
a monotonic and smoothly decreasing. in Fig. 2. Noting the preponderance of walkthroughs among the
nonlocked behaviors at low Fv,, [6],we see that the overall ef-
111. SYNAPTIC MODIFICATION: EFFECTS ON BEHAVIOR
fect will be to make the synaptic mapping more monotonic, as
One advantage of simulation over experiment is that the power far as average rates are concerned.
of the synaptic coupling can be altered. This change in con- For higher values of Po,,, we see pronounced locking tongues,
nectivity can be considered analogous to changing the weight amounting to about one-third of the input rate scale in the
w in a weighted-sum ANN. The resulting behaviors are conve- S A 0 [4]. Varying ps,,,, within this range can have either no
niently summarized by an Arnol’d map [12] or two-dimensional effect or dramatic effect on behavior, as illustrated by the dia-
bifurcation diagram, shown in Fig. 3, which illustrates the type gram of a locking tongue presented in Fig. 4, where the ANN-
of output behavior as a function of input frequency (normal- like terms of w and f have been substituted for Fsy,, and N / I ,
ized as N / I ) and input amplitude (Fv,,, the maximum synaptic respectively. For presynaptic frequencies fi and f2, changing
permeability to Cl-, the inhibitory carrier ion here). the synaptic strength from w1 to w2 has no effect on their cor-
We see in the figure that a locking at any particular ratio (the responding postsynaptic rates: this is true for each frequency
labeled white areas in the graph) occupies a contiguous, veni- fj within the tongue at both WI and w2. For this range,
cally elongated region of the (Po,,, I ) plane; hence their usual A(behavior)/Aclwlf,,, = 0. Conversely, for f 3 , a change from
appellation, tongues. The widths of these tongues vary sys- w1 to w2 induces a change from nonlocked to locked behavior.
-
tematically, according to a Farey series, with the widest tongue This can be true even if w2 w1 is infinitesimallysmall, if f3 is
407
c!ose enough to the bifurcation point represented by the tongue REFERENCES
boundary, Besides the lack of change in behavior within tongues [ I ] J. Segundo, J.-E Vibert, M. Stiber, aiid S . Hanneton,
and the abrupt changes in behavior near tongue boundaries, there “Synaptic coding of periodically modulated spike trains,”
are behavior changes among the nonlocked behaviors between in IEEE International Conference on Neural Networks,
the tongues, which include walkthroughs and messy behaviors 1993.
in about equal proportions in the S A 0 [4].
[2] J. Segundo and D. Perkel, “The nerve cell as an analyzer of
IV CONCLUSIONS
spike trains,” in The interneuron: UCLA Forum in Medi-
Though we cannot state a definitive synaptic modification rule cal Sciences (M. Brazier, ed.),(Los Angeies), pp. 349-89,
currently, the above observations can serve to constrain some University of California Press, 1969.
of its qualities First of all, it is important to note the complex
responses of neurons to inputs with different interspike interval [3] J. P. Segundo, E. Altshuler, M. Stiber, and A. Garfinkel,
patterns, changes in input frequency, and changes in synapse “Periodic inhibition of living pacemaker neurons: I.
power. In recurrent ANNs, the effects of dynamics (such as bi- Locked, intermittent, messy, and hopping behaviors,” Int.
furcation behavior) on learning rules must either be eliminated J. Bifurcation and Chaos, vol. 1, pp. 549-81, September
or carefully controlled to achieve the desired results [13]. It is 1991.
our feeling that a synaptic modification procedure in networks
[4]J. P. Segundo, E. Altshuler, M. Stiber, and A. Garfinkel,
of more realistic elements should take advantage of the individ-
“Periodic inhibition of living pacemaker neurons: U. In-
uals’ dynamics; greater complexity at that level should lead to
fluences of driver rates and transients and of non-driven
greater network computational power. To be far, however, it
post-synaptic rates,” Inf. J. Bzfurcation and Chaos, vol. 1,
could certainly be argued that, in large networks with high in-
pp. 873-90, December 1991.
terconnectivity, the individual elements’ detailed dynamics are
washed out. [ 5 ] J. Segundo, M. Stiber, E. Altshuler, and J.-E Vibert, “Tran-
Based on the previous discussion of coding at small and large sients in the inhibitory driving of neurons and their post-
synaptic strengths, we can make some general conclusions. With synaptic consequences,” Neuroscience, submitted, 1993.
weak coupling, the coding through the synapse is more mono-
tonic, since the locking regions are narrow The tradeoff for this [6] M, Stiber, Dynamrcs of Synaptic Integration. PhD thesis,
is that, as the synapse is made weaker, the effect of input at that University of California, Los Angeles, 1992.
synapse is reduced. Weak synapses would have to gain a notice-
[7] D. Cox and P. Lewis, The Statistical Analysis of Series of
able effect through correlation at multiple sites. However, strict
Events. London: Methuen, 1966.
temporal correlation at multiple sites would tend to produce the
same types of behaviors as a single strong synapse. Therefore, [8] P. Berg4 Y. Pomeau, and C. Vidal, Order Within Chaos: A
for monotonic coding through multiple weak connections, steps Deterministic Approach to Turbulence. Nea York: Wiley,
would need to be taken to reduce synchronization among the 1986.
inputs, so that the postsynaptic unit would respond to overall
input intensity, rather than input pattern. [9] M. Stiber and W. J. Karplus, “Amol’d map synthesis for
With higher values of FJyn (or multiple correlated inputs), periodically forced oscillators,” Tech. Rep. HKUST-CS93-
a complex code can anse, whose functional significance is cur- 5 , HKUST Computer Science Department, 1993.
rently unknown. It is important to note, though, that the arrange-
[IO] M. Stiber and J. P. Segundo, “Nonlinear dynamics and
ment of different behaviors is not random; there is a systematic
complex synaptic transfer functions,” in IEEE Infernational
progression from one to another [4].Potentially, then, this com-
Conference on Neural Networks, 1993.
plexity could have useful computational applications. A learning
scheme at this level of coupling would essentially be adjusting [ I l l G. Sugihara, D.Grace, J. Segundo, and M. Stiber in prepa-
the proportions of the different behaviors, rather than their loca- ration, 1993.
tions along the input rate scale (the latter being determined by
the neural dynamics itself). [12] V. Amol’d, Geometrical Methods in the Theory of Ordinary
We can conclude that any synaptic modification process im- Differential Equations. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1983.
plemented for dynamical, neuron-like elements must take into
[13] K. Doya, “Bifurcations of recurrent neural networks in gra-
account: the tradeoff between synaptic monotonicity and effi-
dient descent learning,” IEEE Transactions on Neural Net-
cacy, synchronizabon and desynchronization of inputs, and con-
works, submitted, 1993.
trol of behavior proportionality. Exactly how these issues should
be dealt with depends on the computational significance of the
different behavior types, a topic of ongoing investigation.