Syntax 2 - Student
Syntax 2 - Student
Syntax 2 - Student
Structural ambiguity
Syntactic categories
Phrase structure trees
Phrase structure rules
/
Syntax: Infinite Use
of Finite Means
It is a remarkable fact that any speaker of a human language can learn and store
in his or her mental lexicon thousands of words, each of which is an arbitrary
pairing of sound and meaning. Even more astonishing is our ability to combine
these words to produce and understand an infinite number of novel sentences,
as we showed with the following sentence:
This is the dog that worried the cat that killed the rat that ate the malt
that lay in the house that Jack built ...
To further illustrate, consider the following:
Snorlax is asleep.
The monster is asleep.
The friend of the monster is asleep.
The rightmost person in the first row is asleep.
The person immediately to the left of the rightmost person in the front row
is asleep.
The person behind the person immediately to the left of the rightmost
person in the first row is asleep.
Snorlax is asleep.
Pikachu noticed that Snorlax is asleep.
75
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Rc~rved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUeated, ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
76 CHAPTER 3 Syntax: Infinite Use of Finite Means
All languages have mechanisms of this sort that make the number of sen-
tences limitless. Like words, discussed in the previous chapter, sentences are
composed of finitely many discrete units that are combined by rules. Thus lan-
guages make infinite use of finite means. In this respect knowledge of language
is like knowledge of integers. There is no limit to the number of even integers
you could enumerate: 2, 4, 6, 8, 10, .... Clearly, you didn't memorize all of them.
Rather, you know a rule that allows you to produce new integers from old ones.
Rule: If E is an integer, E+2 is an integer.
This ability to make infinite use of finite means shows the creative nature of
human linguistic knowledge-not creative in the sense that we are all accom-
plished poets, but creative in that none of us is limited to a fixed repertoire of
expressions. Rather, we can exploit the resources of our language and gram-
mar to produce, understand and make judgments about a limitless number of
sentences embodying a limitless range of ideas and emotions.
The part of grammar that represents a speaker's knowledge of sentences and
their structures is called syntax. The aim of this chapter is to first show you
what syntactic structures look like and then to familiarize you with some of
the rules that determine them. Most of the examples will be from the syntax of
English, but the principles that account for syntactic structures are universal.
The rules of syntax combine words into phrases and phrases into sentences.
Among other things, the rules define the correct word order for a language.
For example, English is a Subject-Ver~bject (SVO) language. The English
sentence in (1) is grammatical because the words occur in the right order; the
sentence in (2) is ungrammatical because the word order is incorrect for English.
1. The President nominated a new Supreme Court justice.
2. *President the Supreme new justice Court a nominated.
The rules of the syntax also specify the grammatical relations of a sentence,
such as subj ect and direct object. In other words, they provide information
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Rc~rved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUeated, ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
78 CHAPTER 3 Syntax: Infinite Use of Finite Means
Sentence Structure
I really do not know that anything has ever been more exciting than diagramming
sentences.
GERTRUOE STEIN, "Poetry and Grammar," 1935
The job of the linguist is to describe the structure of the sentences in a language
in a way that matches the linguistic knowledge of its speakers. We can compare
two competing hypotheses. The first says that a sentence consists simply of a
string of words organized in a flat structure as in (1).
1.
The child found a puppy
We have already seen that word order is an important aspect of syntactic knowl-
edge and this simple diagram correctly captures the SVO word order of English:
The subject (S) the child, comes before the verb (V) found, which comes before
the object (0) a puppy.
Let us contrast this kind of description with another, one that says that sen-
tences have a tree-like structure in which words are grouped together into
natural units nested within other natural units in a hierarchical arrangement,
as in (2).
2. - - - - - root
a puppy
The "tree" in (2) is upside down with its "root" encompassing the entire sen-
tence, "The child found a puppy," and its "leaves" being the individual words
the, child, found, a, and puppy. The tree diagram in (2), embodies the hypothesis
that these words are organized into subunits (or subtrees) and that speakers
mentally represent sentences not as flat strings of words, but as complex struc-
tures with an internal organization. The subunits (or subtrees) of the sentence
are called constituents.
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUcated, ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
80 CHAPTER 3 Syntax: Infinite Use of Finite Means
In the tree diagram in (2), the words a and puppy form a constituent, as
indicated below:
We can also represent constituents by using square brackets around the words
[a puppy]. Constituents can be nested inside one another. So, [a puppy] occurs
inside the constituent [found a puppy], as illustrated in the following tree.
A constituent consists not just of the words, but of the subtree that branches
into the words, and it ends at the node where the branches meet. A constituent
corresponds to a node on the tree. And to be a constituent all the words under
the node must be included. The words that form a constituent are contiguous
(next to one another), but not all contiguous words form a constituent. In the
following tree, the words found a are contiguous but they do not form a constitu-
ent. They are not contained exclusively under the same node.
the child
not a constituent
We began our discussion with a simple sentence "The child found a puppy,"
but this simple sentence belies a complex internal structure. The tree diagram
in (2) groups the words of the sentence into the constituents the child and found
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUcated, ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
Sentence Structure 81
the garden
The move-as-a-unit test can also tell us when what appears to be a con-
stituent, such as a prepositional phrase, is in fact something different. The two
phrases ran up the hill and ran up the bill are superficially quite similar, but we
see in (3) and (4) that they behave quite differently. Consider fJISt the expression
ran up the hill, as in (3a). The rules of the syntax allow the word orders in (3b, c)
as variants, revealing that up the hill is a constituent. By contrast, the expression
n.m up the bill in (4a) does not have these same options, as shown in ( 4b, c),
which means that up the bill is neither a prepositional phrase nor a constituent.
3. (a) Jack ran up the hill.
(b) Up the hill Jack ran.
(c) Up the hill ran Jack.
4. (a) Jack ran up the bill.
(b) *Up the bill Jack ran.
(c) *Up the bill ran Jack.
Structural Ambiguity
DID ~OU r
THE WAI rR€ ~<-"C IT MU<'f NOT HAVE
NO<;€ R tNG "THAT LoUD.
GAP
Syntactic trees reflect our judgments about the internal organization of sen-
tences; flat structures do not. They can also account for other linguistic judg-
ments, such as when a sentence is ambiguous. A sentence is ambiguous if it
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Rc~rved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUcated,ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
Sentence Structure 83
has two or more meanings. Sometimes an ambiguity arises because a word has
more than one meaning, as in the following sentence:
This will make you smart.
The two interpretations of this sentence are due to the two meanings of sman-
"clever" and "burning sensation." This is referred to as a lexical ambiguity
and will be discussed further in Chapter 4. Other times multiple meanings arise
because a sentence has more than one tree structure associated with it, result-
ing in a structural ambiguity. Each tree will correspond to one of the possible
meanings of the sentence. For example, the sentence:
Sue saw the man with the telescope.
has two different meanings:
Meaning 1: The seeing is done with the telescope. 1.She used a telescope to see the man
Meaning 2: The man is holding the telescope. 2 She saw the man who had a telescope
Notice that none of the individual words is ambiguous. The ambiguity is struc-
tural: The sentence has two different trees. Meaning 1 corresponds to the tree
in (1), what we might call the instrumental meaning in which Sue is using the
telescope to see the man. In this tree, the the man and the prepositional phrase
with the telescope do not form a constituent.
1.
- - - not a constituent
Meaning 2 corresponds to the tree in (2). In this case, the phrases the man and
with a telescope do form a constituent, reflecting the meaning in which the man
is holding the telescope.
man - - - a constituent
with the telescop<;
None of these sentences has the interpretation in which the seeing is done
with the telescope. In each, the only possible meaning is that the man is holding
the telescope. This shows us that the structure of a sentence contributes impor·
tantly to its meaning, a point we will come back to in Chapter 4.
Structural ambiguities of the sort just discussed provide striking evidence in
support of our hypothesis that sentences have a tree-like (hierarchical) structure,
and against the idea that they are simply strings of words. The flat structure
hypothesis could not explain how there can be two different meanings associ·
ated with the same string of words.
The cartoon at the head of this section illustrates both lexical and structural
ambiguity. The lexical ambiguity is on the two meanings of ring; the structural
ambiguity is whether nose ring is understood as a compound noun or a noun
followed by a verb.
Syntactic Categories
There are ten parts of speech, and they are all troublesome.
MARK TWAIN, "The Awful German Language; in A TrompAbrood,l880
In the previous section, we illustrated how tree structures reflect our knowledge
of the hierarchical organization of sentences. Speakers also have implicit know!·
edge of the categories of each of the sub groupings in a sentence.
Each grouping in the tree diagrams of "The child found a puppy" is a member
of a large family of similar expressions. For example, the child belongs to a family
that includes the police officer, your neighbor, this yellow cat; he, John, and countless
others. We can substitute any member of this family for the child without affecting
the grarnmaticality of the sentence, although the meaning of course would change.
A police officer found a puppy.
Your neighbor found a puppy.
This yellow cat found a puppy.
A family of expressions that can substitute for one another without loss of
grarnmaticality is called a syntactic category, or more informally, a "part of
speech." The child, a police officer, John, and so on belong to the syntactic category
n oun phrase (NP). NPs may function as subjects or as objects in sentences. An NP
often contains a determiner (such as a or the) and a noun, but it may also consist of
a proper name (Ann), a pronoun (I), a noun without a determiner (fish), or even a
clause or a sentence (that dogs bark). Even though a proper noun such as John and
pronouns such as he and him are single words, they are technically NPs, because
they pattern like NPs in being able to fill a subject, object or other NP slot
John found the puppy.
He found the puppy.
Boys love puppies.
The puppy loved him.
The puppy loved John.
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUcated, ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
Sentence Structure 8.5
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUcated, ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
86 CHAPTER 3 Syntax: Infinite Use of Finite Means
C> The New Yorker Collection 2003 William Haefeli from cartoonbank.com All Rights Reserved.
Syntactic categories include both phrasal categories such as NP, VP, AP (adjec-
tive phrase), PP (prepositional phrase), and AdvP (adverbial phrase), as well as
lexical categories such as noun (N), verb (V), preposition (P), adjective (A), and
adverb (Adv). Each lexical category has a corresponding phrasal category. Fol-
lowing is a list of phrasal categories and lexical categories with some examples
of each type:
Phrasal categories
Noun Phrase (NP) men, the man, the man with a telescope sees, always
Verb Phrase (VP) sees, rarely sees the man, often sees the man with a
telescope
Adjective Phrase (AP) happy, very happy, very happy about winning
Prepositional Phrase over, nearly over, nearly over the hill
(PP)
Adverbial Phrase brighdy, more brighdy, more brighdy than the Sun
(AdvP)
Lexical categories
Noun (N) puppy, boy, man, soup, happiness, fork, kiss, pillow
Verb (V) find, run, sleep, throw, realize, see, try, want, believe
Preposition (P) up, down, across, into, from, by, with, over
Adjective (A) red, big, happy, candid, hopeless, fair, idiotic, lucky
Adverb (Adv) again, always, brighdy, often, never, very, fairly
Many of these categories may already be familiar to you. Other categories
may be less familiar such as the category determiner (Det), which includes
the articles a and the, as well as demonstratives such as this, that, these, and
those, and "quantifiers" such as each and every. Another less familiar category
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUcated,ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
88 CHAPTER 3 Syntax: Infinite Use of Finite Means
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUcated,ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
Sentence Structure 89
Now that you know something about constituent structure and grammatical
categories, you are ready to learn how the phrases and sentences of a language
are constructed. We will begin by illustrating trees for simple phrases and then
proceed to more complex structures. The trees that we will build here are more
detailed than those we saw in the previous sections, because the branches of the
tree will have category labels identifying each constituent. In this section, we
will also introduce the kind of syntactic rules that generate (a technical term
for describe or specify) the different structures.
The tree diagram in (1) provides labels for each of the constituents of the
sentence "The child found a puppy." These labels show that the entire sentence
belongs to the syntactic category of S (because the S-node encompasses all the
words). It also reveals that the child and a puppy belong to the category NP, that
is, they are noun phrases, and that found a puppy belongs to the category VP
or is a verb phrase, consisting of a verb and an NP. It also shows the syntactic
category of each of the words in the sentence.
1. s
~
NP VP
/"'. ~
DetN V NP
I I I /"'.
the child found Det N
I I
a puppy
are immediately dominated by the same node are sisters. V and NP are sisters
in the phrase structure tree of "the child found a puppy."
PS trees are also useful for defining various grammatical relations in a precise
way. For example, the subject of a sentence is the NP immediately dominated
by S (the chUd in the tree in (1) and the direct object is the NP immediately
dominated by VP (the puppy in the tree in (1 ).
s
A
NP VP
I
v
~
------------
NP
~
VP
Det N v pp
I I I ~
the puppy played P NP
I~
m Det N
I I
the garden
C(omp), a functional category like T(ense) and Det. Here is the structure of such
sentence types:
s
NP VP
/'---._
Det N v-------------
CP
I
the
I
professor hoped
I
c
I
------------s
that --------------
NP
/'---._ ~
VP
Det N v NP
I I I ~
the student~ read Det N
I I
the chapter
To allow such embedded sentences, we need to add these two new rules to
our set of phrase structure rules.
7. VP-+ V CP
8. CP .... CS
CP stands for complementizer phrase. Rule 8 says that CP contains a comple-
mentizer such as that followed by the embedded sentence. Other complementiz-
ers are if and whether in sentences such as
I don't know whether I should talk about this.
The teacher asked if the students understood the syntax lesson.
which have structures similar to the one above.
Here are the PS rules we have discussed so far. The rules have been slightly
renumbered to keep all the VP rules together. We will introduce some other
rules later. Phrase structure rules
1. S-+NPVP
2. NP -+ DetN
3. VP-+VNP
4. VP-+V
5. VP-+ VPP
6. VP-+VCP
7. PP .... PNP
8. CP-+CS
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUcated, ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
Sentence Structure 93
The phrase structure rules can be used as a guide for building trees that follow
the structural constraints of the language. In so doing, certain conventions are
followed. The S occurs at the top or "root" of the tree (remember the tree is
upside down). So, first find the rule with Son the left side of the arrow (rule 1)
and put the categories on the right side below the S, as shown here
s
A
NP VP
s
/"---..
NP VP
A.
Det N
The categories at the bottom are Det, N, and VP, but only VP occurs to the left
of an arrow in the set of rules and so needs to be expanded using one of the VP
rules. Any one of the rules will work. The order in which the rules appear in the
list of rules is irrelevant. (We could have begun by expanding the VP rather than
the NP.) Suppose we use rule 4 next. Then, the tree has grown to look like this:
s
~
NP VP
A. A.
DetNVPP
We continue in this way until all phrasal categories are expanded, that is,
none of the categories at the bottom of the tree appears on the left side of any
rule. The PP must expand into a P and an NP (rule 7), and the NP into a Det
and anN. (Proper names and pronouns which are NPs and not nouns are an
Copyright 2019 Cengage Leamlng. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scannoed, or dupUcated, ln whOle or In par1. WCN 02·200-202
94 CHAPTER 3 Syntax: Infinite Use of Finite Means
exception to the "full expansion convention.") We can use a rule as many times
as it can apply. In this tree, we used the NP rule twice. After we have applied
all the rules that can apply, the tree looks like this:
s
~
NP VP
A /"....
Det N V PP
/"....
P NP
A
Det N