MSC Introduction To Syntax: Lecture 1: What Syntax Is About What Is This Module About?
MSC Introduction To Syntax: Lecture 1: What Syntax Is About What Is This Module About?
Such rules are learned consciously, via explicit instruction, and are largely
arbitrary (or sometimes they reflect older stages of the language, such as the
morphological case distinction in (3)). In this respect they resemble rules for
table etiquette or traffic rules.
A descriptivist grammarian will answer the above question by pointing out
that people who have learned English as a first language can tell for any
string of words, such as those in (4)-(10), whether it is a possible English
sentence or not, without having had explicit instruction for this or being
conscious of the rule system that he or she uses to decide on this.
4. a. Mary read a book
b. Mary read which book? (possible, but interpreted as an ‘echo
question’)
c. Which book did Mary read?
d. Mary read a book and the newspaper
e. * Which book did Mary read and the newspaper?
5. a. Who did Mary say that the university gave an Honorary Degree
to yesterday?
c. * Who did Mary day what kind of degree they gave to yesterday?
6. a. John does not like those novels
b. Those novels, John does not like
c. John does not like those novels by Jones
d. * Those novels, John does not like by Jones
7. a. Jane likes reading novels.
b. What does Jane like reading?
c. Reading novels upsets Jane
c. * What does reading upset Jane?
8. a. John is eager to please
a’. What is John eager to do?
b. John is easy to please
b’. * What is John easy to do?
9. a. John wants to shave himself/him (himself = John, him ≠ John)
b. John wants Bill to shave himself/him (himself ≠ John, him = John)
10. etc etc
Note that it is unlikely that a child learns how to distinguish possible from
impossible sentences by instruction, because:
(i) how many children will ever be instructed about facts such as those
in (4)-(9), or even hear such sentences? Yet all English speakers will
agree on their relative grammaticality.
(ii) all children show a similar developmental path in first-language
acquisition, irrespective of the instruction they might get (in fact,
they seem to ignore explicit instruction).
(iii) it is not possible to give a child a list of all possible sentences in a
language, since the number of possible sentences is literally infinite
(see below).
Apparently, every child unconsciously learns a set of rules/principles, with
which it can distinguish the possible sentences in the language it hears
around him/her from the impossible ones. This is a faculty with which all
humans are endowed. (Apparently, there is a critical period for this faculty to
MSc Syntax: 1 3
c. this blue book, that nearby book, a long, tedious, dull, French
book, ...
The sequences in (b) are unacceptable, but are they ungrammatical in the
specialized sense of syntactically ill-formed? Or is the problem rather with the
semantics?
16. Sentence
3
Constituent Constituent
3 3
Constituent Constituent Constituent Constituent
(16) expresses the claim/fact that there is a sentence that consists of two
constituents, which both happen to consist of two smaller constituents.
A simple sentence like The girl read a book will have the tree structure in (17)
(more on this in lectures 3 and 4):
17 S
qo
Noun Phrase Verb Phrase
2 3
Determiner Noun Verb Noun Phrase
3
the girl read Determiner Noun
the book
Recursion
A fundamental property of all natural (non-artificial) languages is that they
have a property called recursion. This is related to what we noted just now:
constituents can contain smaller constituents. Recursion is the phenomenon in
which a constituent can contain a smaller constituent of the same type as the
bigger constituent itself. It is this property that is responsible for the fact
mentioned earlier that the number of possible sentences in a language is
infinite. Thus, a sentence can contain a smaller sentence as one of its
constituents, for instance as its direct object, as in (18b).
18. a. John regrets [this fact]
b. John regrets [that Mary believes these rumours]
Within the smaller sentence the same is possible, and so on, ad infinitum:
19. John regrets [that Mary believes [that Harry has said [that the paper
reported [that…
There is no principled limit to recursion. Of course, for practical reasons any
sentence will come to an end in actual conversations—but grammatically
speaking there is nothing wrong with endlessly recursive structures. This
means that we are making a distinction between what has been called
competence and performance.
Recursion is possible for other sorts of constituents than full sentences as well.
For example, a word group built around a noun, a noun phrase or NP, can
contain within it a group built around a preposition, a preposition phrase or PP:
20. [The dog [in the car]]
But as this example also shows, the PP in the car in turn contains another NP,
namely the car. Nothing stops this NP from containing a PP again, just like the
bigger NP in which it occurs:
MSc Syntax: 1 6
22. NP
9
Det N PP
3
P NP
9
Det N PP
3
P NP
3
Det N
We see that a noun phrase can contain another noun phrase, and a
preposition phrase another preposition phrase. Again, there is no principled
limit to this:
23. The dog in the car next to the garage in the street of the town on the
river …