Limba Engleza Contemporana
Limba Engleza Contemporana
Limba Engleza Contemporana
4. Types of predication
Copulative predication
A copulative predicate consists of a linking verb and a predicative which may be
adjectival or nominal. The adjectival predicative may be realized by an AP. The nominal
predicative may be expressed by a NP, PP or a clause.
The role of the link verb BE as part of the copulative predicate:
a. it connects the subject NP to the NP/AP functioning as predicative
b. it enters into a relationship of agreement in person and number with the subject of the
sentence.
c. it provides information concerning tense and aspect.
There are 2 classes of link verbs:
a. semantically empty link verbs: BE
b. semantically poor verbs: BECOME, APPEAR, GROW, REMAIN, GO, STAND, FALL,
FEEL, GET, RUN, SMELL, TASTE.
Some of these semantically poor verbs can also be used as transitive or intransitive verbs.
The predicative is referentially dependent on the subject of the sentence to which it
gives an attribute or an identity. The main property of the identifying predicative is that it is
reversible that is it can change positions with the subject.
A. The adjectival predicative
Adjectives that are used predicatively may be non-derived or derived. Some predicative
adjectives are derived from transitive verbs by means of suffixation. The DO of the transitive
verb becomes a PO in the copulative predication.
There is a special group of predicative adjectives which indicate state/condition and
which are prefixed by ‘a-’: alike, averse, afraid, akin. These predicative adjectives may take
clausal complement.
The classification of the predicative adjectives according to the type of subject they select
Predicative adjectives may be classified according to the semantic features of the subject
they select:
a. predicative adjectives that select a [+animate] subject [+-human] include: hungry, attentive,
eager, sleepy, tired, playful, alive, wicked
b. predicative adjectives that only combine with a [+human] subject are: sorry, imaginative,
ingenious, kindhearted, polite, sincere
Antonymic pairs: clever/dull, careful/careless, sincere/hypocritical, silent/talkative
c. predicative adjectives that take [+concrete] subject include adjectives denoting colour, shape,
and antonymic pairs of adjectives such as: big/small, long/short, thick/thin, new/old.
d. predicative adjectives that select a [+abstract] subject are: obvious,easy/hard/difficult, simple,
advisable, necessary, natural, likely.
B. The nominal predicative is expressed by a NP, PP or a clause. The nominal predicative
expressed by a NP is always indefinite. The nominal predicative expressed by a PP may be
preceded by the preposition OF or by other prepositions. All prepositions in the English language
can be part of a nominal predicative.
Intransitive predication
Verbs with one argument have been traditionally known as intransitive verbs. Intransitive
verbs obligatorily take a NP in subject position which receives the thematic role of Agent or
Patient. Intransitive verbs are of 2 types: simple and complex.
Semantically simple intransitive verbs express events of all types. From a syntactic
point of view intransitive verbs may optionally take an adverbial modifier.
Classes
1. Phrasal intransitive verbs (V+particle) are followed by particles with various meanings.
Certain particles of phrasal verbs show direction (go up, fly past, fall down). Most of these verbs
indicate motion. Certain particles refer to the temporal dimension of the event. They may suggest
the beginning of the activity in which case the particles are called ingressive (out, about, off, in).
ON and AWAY indicate the continuation of the activity. They convey a durative meaning. The
particle ON may combine freely with any verb. In contrast the particle AWAY cannot be used
with all verbs. The particles OUT, UP and AWAY express the end of the action (+off, through)
that is why they are also called egressive particles.
2. Reflexive intransitive verbs always take a reflexive pronoun which is co-referential with the
subject of the sentence. There is always a relationship of agreement in person and number
between the subject of the sentence and the reflexive pronoun. Almost all reflexive verbs in
English can be used as transitive or intransitive verbs (without the reflexive pronoun).
Prepositional reflexive verbs: Acquaint oneself with smth; abandon oneself to smth; accustom
oneself to smth, adapt onself to smth, congratulate onself on/upon smth; worry onself about/over
smth; revenge onself on smb.
Complex intransitive verbs are two-place predicates that is they obligatorily take a subject and
a prepositional object or an adverbial modifer to form a correct sentence. Classes:
1. Prepositional intransitive verbs
Intransitive verbs belonging to this group always take a prepositional object expressed by
a prepositional phrase. By passivization the object of the preposition is moved in initial sentence
position, while the preposition gets separated from its object and remains attached to the verb.
When we want to emphasize the prepositional object we move it to pre-subject position.
This rearrangement of the constituents of the sentence is called topicalization.
2. Prepositional phrasal verbs
By passivization the object of the preposition becomes subject of the passive sentence
while the particle and the preposition remain in situ. The prepositional object is emphasized by
means of topicalization. The particle remains in situ.
3. Intransitives with IOs
All these verbs take an IO marked by the preposition TO. From a semantic point of view
these verbs are of several types:
-eventive: happen
-verbs of seeming: see, appear
-verbs of mental processes: occur
-verbs of perception: taste, sound
-relational verbs showing possession: to belong and inferiority relations: surrender, submit
4. Intransitives with two PO
A number of complex intransitive verbs may be followed by 2 PP. the 1st PP may
function as an IO or as PO with the semantic feature [+human]. The 2 nd PP functions as a PO and
it indicates the topic of discussion.
5. Complex intransitives with adverbial modifiers: of place, manner; quantifiying
adverbials of place, time, that indicate price.
Transitive predication
Most transitive verbs in English express human activities/events in which human play the
Agent role. Transitive verbs typically take 2 arguments to form a correct sentence: one in subject
position and the other one in object position. The subject NP can be Theme, Patient or
Experiencer. Transitive verbs are of two types: simple and complex. Simple transitive verbs are
of 2 types: two-place predicates or three-place predicates.
Various classifications of simple transitive verbs
1. according to the meaning of the DO selected
a. Certain simple transitive verbs indicate activities associated with affected object: decorate a
house, break a window, brush a hat, burn paper, carry a box, drink milk.
b. simple transitives with resultative objects include verbs that show that the NP functioning as
DO is the result of the action: cook cakes, manufacture goods, produce, create a model, erect a
monument, make a toy. Some transitive verbs with resultative objects may have a [+abstract]/
[+concrete] DO. A special type of effected object is the so called cognate object which is
actually a repetition of the verbal form: smile, drink, love.
c. a small number of simple transitive verbs take a DO which denotes the instrument used to
perform a certain activity: use force/terrorism/backmail; manipulate peole/power/event; play a
record/tape.
d. another group of simple transitive verbs take [-animate] DOs: approach a place/topic; claim a
object/right, analyze a substance/idea, examine a paper/theory.
2. according to the verbal meaning
From a semantic point of view simple transitive verbs may be grouped into three classes:
a. psychological verbs express emotional states, they take an experiencer argument either in
subject position (like, adore, admire, dislike, hate, despise, appreciate, respect) or in object
position (amaze, astonish, surprise, upset, scare, embarrass, bore). Object experiencer verbs may
take a subject which is [+human] or [+abstract].
b. relational verbs express symmetric or asymmetric relations between the subject and the
object of the sentence. Symmetric relations are expressed by reciprocal verbs: resemble, meet.
The asymmetric relations are those expressing inclusion, possession or acquisition. Inclusion
relations are expressed by transitive verbs such as: contain, hold, comprise, include, cover. Verbs
such as: have, own, possess indicate alienable or inalienable possession. With verbs of
acquisition (get, acquire, receive, appropriate) the subject denotes the Beneficiary and the DO
shows the Patient/Theme of the action.
c. causative verbs are transitive verbs that express direct causation of an event or an event in
which causation is implied. From a syntactic point of view causative constructions are all
transitive owing to the fact that causation implies 2 participants: a causer and an affected/effected
entity. They are of 3 types:
1. periphrastic verbs include: cause, determine, make, have, get which have a very
general causative meaning. Semantically these verbs express the idea of causation quite
neutrally. They are often associated with [+abstract] nouns. The verbs HAVE and GET
additionally convey the idea of obligation/order.
2. lexical verbs are transitive verbs which can be paraphrased by means of the verb TO
CAUSE: to teach = to cause smb to learn, to convince= to cause smb to believe, to give= to
cause smb to take smth
3. morphological verbs are derived either from adjectives or from nouns. Many
causative transitives are converted from adjectives. The word formation processes are:
A. conversion: Adjectives→Causative verb: bare, better, blind, clean, empty
B. affixation produces causative verbs by attaching prefixes/suffixes or both of them to
adjectives.
Prefixation: dis+able/content/quiet; en+large/rich/tame
Suffixation: happy/solid+ify; American/civil/functional/commercial/legal/popular+ize;
broad/dark/short/white/hard+en
Similar causative transitive verbs can be produced from nouns by means of the same
word formation processes.
A. conversion: noun→verb: age, plant, decay, cream, ornament
B. prefixation: de + colour/forest/form; dis + cart/cover/ credit/favour/ honour/illusion/
interest/order/place; en+cage/circle/code/slave/title
Suffixation: person/beauty+ify; carbon/computer/robot/standard+ize.
Sometimes both prefixation and suffixation are used to produce causative verbs:
deactivate, decentralize, demobilize, demolize, enlighten, disorientated, invalidate.
Auxiliary verbs
Indicate aspect and voice. The auxiliary HAVE followed by the past participle of the
lexical verb shoes perfect aspect. The auxiliary BE occurs with the present participle of the
lexical verbs to mark the progressive aspect and with the past participle to show passive voice.
The auxiliary DO is a verb which helps or supports certain syntactic processes of the
lexical verb where there is no already available auxiliary (the the lexical verb is in the present or
past simple). The auxiliary DO is required to form interrogative and negative sentences when the
verb is in the present or past simple. Because inversion oo the lexical verb with the subject is not
possible the auxiliary DO is inserted in the sentence to form yes or no, wh-, and tag-questions.
Because the lexical verb cannot be followed by NOT we insert the auxiliary DO to form negative
sentences. The auxiliary DO is used not only in interrogation and negation but also in ellipsis and
emphasis.
The auxiliary DO is used to avoid repetition in short answers to yes or no questions, in
coordinate clauses affirmative or negative and in comparative clauses introduced by than.
The auxiliary DO is used as a means of emphasizing in the following contexts:
a. DO is inserted in a positive affirmative statement which appears in contrast with a negative
statement.
b. DO co-occurs with the negative adverb NEVER to indicate emphasis.
c. DO appears in the main clause when this clause stands in contrast with a clause of concession.
d. DO co-occurs with emphatic adverbs such as: certainly, definitely, positively
e. DO is also used in emphatic imperatives especially in BrE.
Passivization
Existential constructions
Sentences containing the expletive pronoun THERE as anticipatory subject are known as
existential constructions. Such constructions are produced by moving the logical subject after the
verb TO BE and by inserting in initial position the expletive pronoun THERE. The resulting
construction contains two subjects: a grammatical one and a real/logical one.
The main characteristic of the logical subject in existential constructions is that it should
be indefinite (preceded by indefinite determiners).
The most frequently used verb in existential constructions is the verb TO BE.
Additionally a number of intransitive verbs can also appear in such configurations.
Intransitive verbs used in existential constructions are of several types:
a. existential verbs: to be, to happen, to occur, to exist, and to live
b. aspectual verbs: to seem and to appear
c. ingressive verbs (the beginning of an action): to emerge, to burst and to arise
d. motion verbs: to come, to arrive and to run
e. positional verbs: to stand, to lie and to hang.
There are two types of constructions that cannot be turned into existential constructions:
the copulative predication and the transitive predication.
6. Interrogative sentences
A negative yes or no question has in initial position the auxiliary and the negator in a
contracted form. In very formal BrE some speakers allow the uncontracted form to appear in
such questions: Has not the prime minister attended the press conference? Such a question is
more likely to be rhetorical rather than information seeking.
*Has J not attended the course?
If the auxiliary and the negator do not appear in a contracted form, then the auxiliary
moves to the initial position in the question without the negator and the result is an
ungrammatical question.
Indirect questions are questions embedded in the structure of another clause. The main
difference in structure between direct and indirect questions is that in indirect questions there is
no inversion between the auxiliary or the modal and the subject of the sentence. However
speakers of certain American dialects allow inversion in indirect questions.
Yes or no indirect questions are introduced by the conjunctions whether/if. However the
use of IF is more restrictive than the use of whether. When the indirect question functions as the
subject for the main clause IF cannot replace WHETHER. Only WHETHER can introduce NF
indirect questions.
The common characteristics of yes or no questions and wh-questions are:
1. the rules of the SQT must be obeyed just like in all other reported speech contexts.
2. there is a change in pronouns from the 1 st/2nd person to the 3rd, and there is also a change in the
adverbs of time.
Alternative yes or no questions
IF can replace WHETHER in indirect alternative questions, the result being less formal.
Sometimes alternative questions may contain a repetition of the first alternative in the negative.
The reduced negative alternative OR NOT can appear either at the end of the sentence or at the
beginning of the indirect question between the conjunction whether and the first alternative. The
reduced negative alternative OR NOT is only allowed in final position when the indirect question
is introduced by the conjunction IF.
Long wh-movement
Complex sentences with multiple subordination can be turned into wh-questions, cleft
constructions or relative clauses by using long wh-movement.
The distinction between short wh-movement and long wh-movement is that wh-
movement is short when the wh-word remains inside the boundaries of the same clause. When
the wh-word crosses several clause boundaries to get to initial position in the main clause then
we speak about long wh-movement. The moved wh-word leaves behind a co-indexed trace
which indicates the basic position of the displaced constituent (wh-word).
Cleft sentence structures are used to focus attention on certain constituents. If we want to
emphasize a constituent we place it after the copulative verb BE while the rest of the basic
sentence is attached as a RC to the emphasized constituent. The relative pronoun WHO moves to
the front position of the RC crossing several clause boundaries.
When the RC is successively subordinated to other TCCs, the relative pronoun WHO
moves from its basic position to the front position in the RC again by means of long wh-
movement.(leaves a co-indexed trace)
7. Complementation
That complement clauses are introduced by the complementizer THAT. TCCs have a number
of syntactic properties that indicate that these clauses are similar to NPs:
1. just like NPs, TCCs may have the syntactic functions of subject, DO, PO, predicative or
attribute.
2. TCCs pronominalize like NPs that is just like NPs, TCCs can be replaced by the pronoun IT or
by the demonstrative THAT.
3. sentences with the structure: Grammatical subject + BE +emphasized constituent + RC (cleft
sentences) are often used for the purpose of emphasizing a constituent.
The NP can be emphasized in a pseudo-cleft sentence (RC + BE + emphasized
constituent). TCCs may appear after the copulative BE in pseudo-cleft constructions just like
NPs.
4. TCCs may appear in constructions with the word THAN which is typically flanked by
constituents of the same type.
5. Extraposition is a syntactic process by means of which a TCC is moved to the end of the
sentence leaving an empty position which is occupied by the expletive pronoun IT. Extraposition
is of 3 types depending on the position from where the TCC is moved: from subject position,
from DO position, from PO position. As a result of this movement of the TCC the sentence has 2
subjects/DOs/POs one of them formal (IT) and the other one logical.
Heavy NP shift involves movement of a very ‘heavy’(long) or complex NP to the end of the
sentence (without inserting the pronoun IT). The TCC can be moved over an adverbial phrase or
over a PP (this PP can be either IO or PO).
3. TCC=predicative when the main clause contains a [+abstract] subject followed by the
copulative verb BE
4. TCC=attribute for [+abstract] nouns in the main clause. The attributive TCCs may be
separated from the abstract noun in highly emphatic statements. These are rare constructions. As
a result of the separation the abstract noun moves to the beginning being interpreted as an
emphasized constituent and the lexical verb is inverted with the subject.
8. NF clauses. RC.
3. ICC=PO when the main clause contains a verb/adjective with obligatory preposition.
Verbs: apply for, consent to, insist on, bother about, long for, care for, fail in
When the ICC=PO then the preposition must be deleted.
Adjectives: sorry for, careful about, afraid of, anxious about, angry at.
5. ICC=attribute for the [+abstract] nouns in the main clause: ability, capacity, wish, idea,
power, right, instinct.
6. ICC=adverbial modifiers
Gerundial clauses
Traditional grammars acknowledge the existence of at least two homonymous ING
forms: the gerund and the present participle. Present participles have verbal and adjectival
features while gerunds have verbal and nominal features. Gerunds are further subdivided into:
verbal gerunds (gerunds proper) and nominal gerunds (verbal noun).
The syntactic analysis of verbal gerunds identifies 3 gerundial constructions: an older one
whose subject is in the genitive case called possessive-ing (full gerund); a construction which
appeared later in the history of English language whose subject is in the accusative case which is
called Accusative-ing (half gerund); the subjectless gerund whose PRO subject is interpreted as
arbitrary called PRO-ing complement.
Nominal gerund
The Ingof is actually a noun derived from a verb by means of the nominalizing suffix
ING. INg turns a verb into a noun just like all the other nominalizing suffixes in English (-tion,
-ment,-ance). The Ingof has all the characteristics of nouns. The structure of the nominal gerund
resembles the structure of nominalizations. The verbal noun is preceded by a determiner,
adjective just like ordinary nouns and it takes the object in a phrase introduced by the preposition
of (of-phrase). Semantically the verbal noun denotes an action/activity that is why it is also
called action nominalization.
Verbal gerund
Verbal gerunds have the structure of clauses (S+V+DO). Gerunds have aspect which is a
typical verbal property. In contrast nominal gerunds do not have aspect.
The participle
The participle has verbal and adjectival properties.
The syntactic functions of the participle
1. predicative
2. attribute coming from a reduced RC are allowed to appear either in pronominal or postnominal
position.
3. the present/past participle can be part of the complex object construction (traditionally known
as the Acc+Present/past pasticiple)[this is the result of SOR].
4. adverbial modifiers of 3 types coming from the reduction of adverbial clauses of time, cause
or manner.
5. participles can be used in parenthetical phrases which express the speakers point of
view/opinion.
Relative clauses
RC also called adjective clauses are embedded clauses linked to a noun from the main
clause frequently by means of a relative pronoun. The noun described by the RC is analyzed as
an antecedent of the relative pronoun. The relative pronoun and the antecedent have the same
referent. That is they are co-referential and this is indicated by the common index attached to
them.
The antecedent and the RC form a complex NP in which the RC functions as adjunct
(optional constituent).
Types of RCs
RC can be classified on semantic and syntactic criteria.
Semantically RCs are of 2 types:
a) RRCs also known as defining RCs are clauses which delimit or identify the antecedent.
Reference is made only to the things described in the RC.
b) NRRCs also known as appositive RCs add suplimentary information about an already
identified antecedent. The identity of the person is given, the RC only gives additional
information about him.
Syntactically RCs are of 2 types:
a) with an expressed antecedent (dependent RCs). Both RRCs and NRRCs have expressed
antecedents in the main clause.
b) without an expressed antecedent (independent RCs). They are understood as describing an
antecedent expressed by an indefinite pronoun. Because this indefinite pronoun is vague it is
omitted an that is why this RCs have an omitted antecedent.
The wh-phrase used in the RC has the same syntactic functions as the NP which was replaced
by it. RCs can be introduced by:
a. relative pronouns (who, what, which)
b. possessive determiner ‘whose’ used before nouns
c. the complementizer that
d. adverbs: when, where, why
When the RC contain a prepositional verb then there are 2 versions of the same RC: one
in which the preposition accompanied the relative pronoun to the front position in the RC(pied-
piping) and the other one in which the preposition remain in situ only the relative pronouns
moves to the beginning of the RC (preposition stranding).
Depending on the verbal form RCs are finite and non-finite.
NFRCs
1. Infinitival RCs are obtained by deleting/omitting the relative pronoun and the auxiliary from
a full finite RC.
2. Participial RCs are derived by omitting the relative pronoun and the verb BE as an auxiliary
from a finite RC with the verb in the progressive aspect.
Participial RCs may also correspond to verbs in the present/past simple from the FRC. Participial
RCs may also correspond to verbs in the passive voice from a full FRC. NFRCs containing a past
participle are reductions of FRCs with the verb in the passive voice.
Free RCs do not have an expressed antecedent in the main clause. They perform all the syntactic
functions of the NPs (S, DO, IO, PO, predicative, attribute). Free RCs are introduced by simple
pronouns/adverbs or by compounds with EVER: indefinite relative pronouns (whichever,
whoever,whatever) and indefinite adverbs (whenever, wherever, however). RCs are also used in
cleft and pseudo-cleft constructions. Cleft constructions rearrange the constituents of a simple
declarative sentence for the purpose of emphasizing one of the constituents and drawing
attention.
9. Speech Act Theory
How to do things with words, Austin 1955. Austin argued that some ordinary declarative
sentences, contrary to logical positivist assumptions, are not used with any intention of making
true or false statements. These sentences are used to do things. Austin refers to them and the
utterances realized by than as perfomatives. Performatives, unlike constatatives which can be
assessed in terms of truth and falsity, cannot be true or false. They can be unhappy or
infelicitous.
The felicity conditions are:
A. (i) There must be a conventional procedure having a conventional effect.
(ii) The circumstances and persons must be appropriate, as specified in the procedure.
B. The procedure must be executed (i) correctly and (ii) completely.
C. the person must have the requisite thoughts, feelings and intentions as specified in the
procedure, and if consequent conduct is specified then the relevant parties must do so.
Performatives are first person indicative active sentences in the simple present tense.
In terms of structure, Austin isolates three basic senses in which in saying something
one is doing something, and hence three kinds of acts that are simultaneously performed:
(i) the locutionary act, having a locutionary meaning, ca be defined as the utterance of a
sentence with a specific sense and reference.
(ii) the illocutionary, having an illocutionary force is the making of a statement, offer, promise
in uttering a sentence by virtue of the conventional force associated with it, or with its explicit
performative phrase.
(iii) the perlocutionary act, having a perlocutionary effect, is the bringing about of effects on the
audience by means of uttering the sentence.
Searle’s taxonomy – 5 types of illocutionary acts:
Representatives/assertives – represent a state of affairs
Directives – direct the addressee towards doing something
Commisives – speakers commit themselves to some future action
Expressives – state what the speaker feels
Declarations – change the world via their utterance
Indirect speech acts
10. Presupposition
Non-factive presuppositions are associated with verbs like dream, imagine, pretend. This verbs
are used with the presupposition that what follows is not true.
I dreamt that I was rich. >> I was not rich.
Second type if-clauses are associated with counter-factual presuppositions, meaning that what
is presupposed is not only true, but it is the opposite of what is true.
If you were my friend, you would have helped. >> You are not my friend.
Presupposition triggers
a. definite descriptions: john saw the man with two heads. >> There exists a man with two
heads.
b. factive verbs: Martha regrets going to john’s party. >> Martha went to john’s party.
c. implicative verbs: john managed to open the door. >> John tried to open the door.
d. change of state verbs: john stopped beating his children. >> John had been beating his
children.
e. iteratives: Clinton returned to power. >> Clinton held power before.
f. temporal clauses: Since C. died we’ve lacked a leader. >> C. died.
g. NRRC: H., who climbed the everest in 1953, was the greatest explorer of our day. >> H.
climbed everest in 1953.
Pragmatic presupposition
Pragmatic presuppositions are related to the context of utterance. Pragmatic presuppositions are
determined by a combination of linguistic item and what we know about the world.
Tell Madonna I’m at lunch. >> Madonna is likely to appear soon. H knows who she is. H will
pass the message on.