Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Skip to main content
Marco Sarà

Marco Sarà

  • Education The University of Genova, School of medicine, M.D. (84-91) Degree of Medicine & Surgery 91, University of ... moreedit
  • Associated editor of Frontiers In Neurology, Reviewer for many indexed journals edit
There is much evidence to suggest that recognizing and sharing emotions with others require a first-hand experience of those emotions in our own body which, in turn, depends on the adequate perception of our own internal state... more
There is much evidence to suggest that recognizing and sharing emotions with others require a first-hand experience of those emotions in our own body which, in turn, depends on the adequate perception of our own internal state (inter-oception) through preserved sensory pathways. Here we explored the contribution of interoception to first-hand emotional experiences and to the recognition of others' emotions. For this aim, 10 individuals with sensory deafferentation as a consequence of high spinal cord injury (SCI; five males and five females; mean age, 48 – 14.8 years) and 20 healthy subjects matched for age, sex, and education were included in the study. Recognition of facial expressions and judgment of emotionally evocative scenes were investigated in both groups using the Ekman and Friesen set of Pictures of Facial Affect and the International Affective Picture System. A two-way mixed analysis of variance and post hoc comparisons were used to test differences among emotions and groups. Compared with healthy subjects, individuals with SCI, when asked to judge emotionally evocative scenes, had difficulties in judging their own emotional response to complex scenes eliciting fear and anger, while they were able to recognize the same emotions when conveyed by facial expressions. Our findings endorse a simulative view of emotional processing according to which the proper perception of our own internal state (interoception), through preserved sensory pathways, is crucial for first-hand experiences of the more primordial emotions, such as fear and anger.
Research Interests:
Cases of recovery from vegetative and minimally conscious state after the administration of various pharmacological agents have been recently reported. These agents include CNS depressants (zolpidem, baclofen, lamotrigine) and CNS... more
Cases of recovery from vegetative and minimally conscious state after the administration of various pharmacological agents have been recently reported. These agents include CNS depressants (zolpidem, baclofen, lamotrigine) and CNS stimulants (tricyclic anti-depressants, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, dopaminergic agents, methylphenidate). The action of CNS depressants as awakening agents sounds paradoxical, as they are commonly prescribed to slow down brain activity in the management of anxiety, muscle tension, pain, insomnia and seizures. How these drugs may improve the level of consciousness in some brain-injured patients is the subject of intense debate. Here we hypothesize that CNS depressants may promote consciousness recovery by reversing a condition of GABA impairment in the injured brain, restoring the normal ratio between synaptic excitation and inhibition, which is the prerequisite for any transition from a resting state to goal-oriented activities (GABA impairment hypothesis). Alternative or complementary mechanisms underlying the improvement of consciousness may include the reversal of a neurodormant state within areas affected by diaschisis (diaschisis hypothesis) and the modulation of an informative overload to the cortex as a consequence of filter failure in the injured brain (informative overload hypothesis). A better understanding of how single agents act on neural networks, whose functioning is critical for recovery, may help to advance a tailored pharmacological approach in the treatment of severely brain injured patients.
Research Interests:
Research Interests:
Locked-in syndrome (LIS) following ventral brainstem damage is the most severe form of motor disability. Patients are completely entrapped in an unresponsive body despite consciousness is preserved. Although the main feature of LIS is... more
Locked-in syndrome (LIS) following ventral brainstem damage is the most severe form of motor disability. Patients are completely entrapped in an unresponsive body despite consciousness is preserved. Although the main feature of LIS is this extreme motor impairment, minor non-motor dysfunctions such as motor imagery defects and impaired emotional recognition have been reported suggesting an alteration of embodied cognition, defined as the effects that the body and its performances may have on cognitive domains. We investigated the presence of structural cortical changes in LIS, which may account for the reported cognitive dysfunctions. For this aim, magnetic resonance imaging scans were acquired in 11 patients with LIS (6 males and 5 females; mean age: 52.3±5.2SD years; mean time interval from injury to evaluation: 9±1.2SD months) and 44 healthy control subjects matching patients for age, sex and education. Freesurfer software was used to process data and to estimate cortical volumes in LIS patients as compared to healthy subjects. Results showed a selective cortical volume loss in patients involving the superior frontal gyrus, the pars opercularis and the insular cortex in the left hemisphere, and the superior and medium frontal gyrus, the pars opercularis, the insular cortex, and the superior parietal lobule in the right hemisphere. As these structures are typically associated with the mirror neuron system, which represents the neural substrate for embodied simulation processes, our results provide neuroanatomical support for potential disembodiment in LIS
Can reading others' emotional states be shaped by expertise? We assessed processing of emotional facial expressions in professional actors trained either to voluntary activate mimicry to reproduce character's emotions (as foreseen by the... more
Can reading others' emotional states be shaped by expertise? We assessed processing of emotional facial expressions in professional actors trained either to voluntary activate mimicry to reproduce character's emotions (as foreseen by the "Mimic Method"), or to infer others' inner states from reading the emotional context (as foreseen by "Stanislavski Method"). In explicit recognition of facial expressions (Experiment 1), the two experimental groups differed from each other and from a control group with no acting experience: the Mimic group was more accurate, whereas the Stanislavski group was slower. Neither acting experience, instead, influenced implicit processing of emotional faces (Experiment 2). We argue that expertise can selectively influence explicit recognition of others' facial expressions, depending on the kind of "emotional expertise"
Research Interests:
The mass media have recently pointed out the likelihood of diagnostic errors in post-coma patients. Late recoveries of consciousness, even after 20 years, might indicate hidden misdiagnoses that are not corrected over a long period of... more
The mass media have recently pointed out the likelihood of diagnostic errors in post-coma patients. Late recoveries of consciousness, even after 20 years, might indicate hidden misdiagnoses that are not corrected over a long period of time. The rate of misdiagnoses of patients in a vegetative state is very high when based on behavioral assessment strategies alone. An extremely restrictive motor repertoire, as occurs in locked-in patients, seems to be the major factor responsible for diagnostic confusion. Functional neuroimaging techniques are regarded as promising tools in unearthing covert awareness in behaviorally unresponsive patients who are unable to produce any motor output. However, unless we believe that these patients persistently live in an unconvincing Cartesian-like state, in which thinking and acting are mutually dissociated, we have to admit that a new taxonomy for low responsive states is called for. This taxonomy should take into account the possible syndromic overlap between disorders of consciousness and locked-in syndrome. We should suspect a "locked-in state" in behaviorally unresponsive patients unless we reach strong evidence that such is not the case; this is the only way to avoid dramatic misdiagnoses.
Objective: To investigate whether corticomotor facilitation induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS-CF) could evoke a simple purposeful motor behavior in patients with a diagnosis of vegetative state. Design: Cross-sectional... more
Objective: To investigate whether corticomotor facilitation induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS-CF) could evoke a simple purposeful motor behavior in patients with a diagnosis of vegetative state.
Design: Cross-sectional survey.
Setting: Post-coma and rehabilitation care unit.
Participants: Patients (NZ6) with a diagnosis of vegetative state.
Interventions: A cascade of consecutive motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) was elicited under 3 different conditions: in the first condition, patients were at rest (Rest); in the second, they were asked to open and close the right hand (Execution); in the third, the examiner modeled a movement of abduction of the thumb in front of the patient who was encouraged in advance to imitate the action (Observation to Imitate).
Main Outcome Measures: Changes in MEP values from the abductor pollicis brevis muscle and improvement in scores on the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised.
Results: TMS-CF alone or combined with verbal instructions did not yield any change; only the combination with imitation caused changes in MEPs (shorter latency and increased amplitude) associated with behavioral improvement in 4 patients.
Conclusions: Encouraging observation to imitate may favor the transformation of some perceived actions into motor images and performances, probably depending on the activation of mirror motor neurons. In our opinion, combining visual input with TMS-CF might have reinforced the coupling between movement planning and execution, promoting the recovery of elementary motor activities in some patients. The proposed protocol may contribute to unmasking signs of preserved consciousness in patients with latent capacities for recovery.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2013
Research Interests:
One of the most controversial issues in the management of patients in a vegetative state or a minimally conscious state concerns their hypothetical capacity to continue to experience pain despite an apparent absence of self- and... more
One of the most controversial issues in the management of patients in a vegetative state or a minimally conscious state concerns their hypothetical capacity to continue to experience pain despite an apparent absence of self- and environmental awareness. Recent functional neuroimaging studies have shown a greater perception of pain in patients in minimally conscious state compared with patients in vegetative state, suggesting the possible involvement of preserved cognitive mechanisms in the process of pain modulation in the former. In addition, a subgroup of patients might continue to experience some elementary emotional and affective feelings, as suggested by the reported activation of specific cerebral areas in response to situations, which commonly generate empathy. However, the available evidence is not sufficient to draw conclusions about the presence or absence of pain experience in patients with disorders of consciousness. Future studies should contribute to a better understanding of which central neural pathways are involved in the perception and modulation of pain in healthy subjects and in patients with severe brain injuries. Such studies should thus also improve our know-how about pain management in this particularly challenging group of patients. © 2013 Springer Science+Business Media New York.
View
Research Interests:
to the Editors: I read with interest the article by Machado and colleagues in the January 2012 issue of MEDICC Review (A Cuban Perspective on Management of Persistent Vegetative State). There are two main ways of considering vegetative... more
to the Editors:

I read with interest the article by Machado and colleagues in the January 2012 issue of MEDICC Review (A Cuban Perspective on Management of Persistent Vegetative State). There are two main ways of considering vegetative state: as a pathology of consciousness per se or as a multifaceted ensemble of different neurological syndromes. As reported in their review, the identification of both anatomical and functional impairment may be—and in our opinion must be—the first step in the assessment of these challenging patients. In our work at San Raffaele Cassino Hospital, we see the vegetative state as a matryoshka [a Russian nesting doll—Eds.] syndrome with a pattern of lesions that is rarely the same from one patient to another. All aspects of impairment, or spared functioning, should, therefore, be examined, from motor potential to higher cognitive functions.

In the literature, ​we are seeing a growing emphasis on connectivity as the keystone in the structural and functional foundations of consciousness. However, this conceptual framework seems to collapse in the case of vegetative or minimally conscious states. In a clinical setting, various means of assessment are generally available to us, including functional MRI with various stimuli; standard MRI to assess extent of brain damage; and neurophysiological assessments using very refined techniques of quantitative analysis.

Nonetheless it is rare to read a paper describing both structural lesions and functional aspects in the same cohort of subjects. Failure to do this leads to serious limitations, since neurophysiological data are thus rarely compared with structural data. Functional MRI findings are generally reported in isolation, without corresponding information concerning whether or not the supporting brain structures are anatomically (as opposed to functionally) compromised. In conclusion, we believe there is a need to reinforce anatomical study as the first step in both clinical practice and science. The work of Machado et al. exemplifies this new and necessary approach—starting from the anatomical picture.

Marco Sarà MD
Post-Coma Intensive Care and Rehabilitation Unit San Raffaele Cassino Hospital Cassino, Italy

Calixto Machado Responds for the Authors

We appreciate Dr Sarà’s comments and agree that the literature has a plethora of articles about disorders of consciousness (DOC) emphasizing isolated results from highly refined techniques of quantitative brain function analysis, and that inability to compare functional data with structural-anatomic information is very limiting.[1]

As Dr Sarà affirms, anatomical lesions in DOC are always a mixture of different patterns and vary from patient to patient.[2] Thus, his matryoshka metaphor for lesions found in persistent vegetative state cases is quite apt.

Most authors overemphasize connectivity as the structural and functional mechanism of consciousness generation, [3] but it is only part of the picture. Although it is very important to assess connectivity between thalamus and neocortex in studying DOC, the presence of anatomical—sometimes sizable—lesions in the brainstem, thalamus, or cerebral hemispheres, plays a key role in explaining consciousness impairment. That is, it is important to assess more than connectivity, because grey matter lesions—which usually also involve neighboring white matter disruption—are also fundamental in DOC pathophysiology.[2]

We agree entirely with Dr Sarà that identifying and correlating both anatomical and functional impairments are necessary to recognize, assess and explain DOC in these challenging patients. And yes, the first step is the anatomical picture.

Sarà M, Pistoia F. Defining consciousness: lessons from patients and modern techniques.J Neurotrauma. 2010 Apr;27(4):771–3.
Machado C. Persistent vegetative and minimally conscious states. Rev Neurosci. 2009;20(3–4):203–20.
Vanhaudenhuyse A, Noirhomme Q, Tshibanda LJ, Bruno MA, Boveroux P, Schnakers C, et al. Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients. Brain. 2010 Jan;133(Pt 1):161–71.

April–July 2017, Vol 19, No 2–3 » Letters to the Editor
The aim of this study was to identify the impact of comorbidities on outcomes of patients with vegetative state (VS) or minimally conscious state (MCS). All patients in VS or MCS consecutively admitted to two postacute care units within a... more
The aim of this study was to identify the impact of comorbidities on outcomes of patients with vegetative state (VS) or minimally conscious state (MCS). All patients in VS or MCS consecutively admitted to two postacute care units within a 1-year period were evaluated at baseline and at 6 months through the Coma Recovery Scale–Revised Version and the Disability Rating Scale (DRS). Comorbidities were also recorded for each patient along the same period. Six-month outcomes included death, full recovery of consciousness, and functional improvement. One hundred and thirty-nine patients (88 male and 51 female; median age, 59 years) were included. Ninety-seven patients were in VS (70%) and 42 in MCS (30%). At 6 months, 33 patients were dead (24%), 39 had a full recovery of consciousness (28%), and 67 remained in VS or MCS (48%). According to DRS scores, 40% of patients (n = 55) showed a functional improvement in the level of disability. One hundred and thirty patients (94%) showed at least one comorbidity. Severity of comorbidities (hazard ratio [HR] = 2.8; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.71–4.68; p < 0.001) and the presence of ischemic or organic heart diseases (HR = 2.6; 95% CI, 1.21–5.43; p = 0.014) were the strongest predictors of death, together with increasing age (HR = 1.0; 95% CI, 1.0–1.06; p = 0.033). Respiratory diseases and arrhythmias without organic heart diseases were negative predictors of full recovery of consciousness (odds ratio [OR] = 0.3; 95% CI, 0.12–0.7; p = 0.006; OR = 0.2; 95% CI, 0.07–0.43; p < 0.001) and functional improvement (OR = 0.4; 95% CI, 0.15–0.85, p = 0.020; OR = 0.2; 95% CI, 0.08–0.45; p < 0.001). Our data show that comorbidities are common in these patients and some of them influence recovery of consciousness and outcomes.
Research Interests:
Disorders of consciousness (DOCs) include coma, vegetative state (VS), and minimally conscious state (MCS). Coma is characterized by impaired wakefulness and consciousness , while VS and MCS are defined by lacking or discontinuous... more
Disorders of consciousness (DOCs) include coma, vegetative state (VS), and minimally conscious state (MCS). Coma is characterized by impaired wakefulness and consciousness , while VS and MCS are defined by lacking or discontinuous consciousness despite recovered wakefulness. Conversely, locked-in syndrome (LIS) is characterized by quadriplegia and lower cranial nerve paralysis with preserved consciousness. Intrathecal baclofen (ITB) is a useful treatment to improve spasticity both in patients with DOCs and LIS. Moreover, it supports the recovery of consciousness in some patients with VS or MCS. The precise mechanism underlying this recovery has not yet been elucidated. It has been hypothesized that ITB may act by reducing the overload of dysfunc-tional sensory stimuli reaching the injured brain or by stabilizing the imbalanced circadian rhythms. Although the current indication of ITB is the management of severe spasticity, its potential use in speeding the recovery of consciousness merits further investigation.
Research Interests:
Primary objective: To reveal covert abilities in a minimally conscious state (MCS) through an innovative activation paradigm based on olfactory imagery. Research design: Case study. Methods and procedures: A patient in MCS was asked to... more
Primary objective: To reveal covert abilities in a minimally conscious state (MCS) through an innovative activation paradigm based on olfactory imagery.

Research design: Case study.

Methods and procedures: A patient in MCS was asked to ‘imagine an unpleasant odour’ or to ‘relax’ in response to the appearance on a screen of a downward pointing arrow or a cross, respectively. Electrophysiological responses to stimuli were investigated by means of an 8-channel EEG equipment and analyzed​ using a specific threshold algorithm. The protocol was repeated for 10 sessions separated from each other by 2 weeks. Accuracy, defined as the number of successes with respect to the total number of trials, was used to evaluate the number of times in which the classification strategy was successful.

Main outcomes and results: Analyses of accuracy showed that the patient was able to activate and to relax himself purposefully and that he optimized his performances with the number of sessions, probably as a result of training-related improvements.

Conclusions: Subtle signs of consciousness may be under-estimated and need to be revealed through specific activation tasks. This paradigm may be useful to detect covert signs of consciousness, especially when patients are precluded from carrying out more complex cognitive tasks.
Research Interests:
Research Interests:
We read with interest the recently published paper about the potential role of " embodied medicine " (Riva et al., 2017). Authors suggest the use of advanced technologies for altering the experience of being in a body, with the goal of... more
We read with interest the recently published paper about the potential role of " embodied medicine " (Riva et al., 2017). Authors suggest the use of advanced technologies for altering the experience of being in a body, with the goal of improving the well-being of patients. This paradigm is intriguingly summarized through the key message " Mens Sana in Corpore Virtuale Sano " and is recommended for patients with different neurological and psychiatric disorders including neglect, chronic pain, schizophrenia, depression and eating disorders. Here we report about a neurological syndrome which, in our opinion, might greatly benefit from the proposed approach and from simulation/stimulation technologies able to modulate the inner body dimension. This is the Locked-in Syndrome (LIS) characterized by a condition of severe motor entrapment due to the interruption of corticospinal, corticobulbar and cortico-cerebellar pathways as a result of a ventral brainstem lesion (Figures 1A,B). Patients are completely entrapped within their body because of quadriplegia, anarthria and lower cranial nerve paralysis, and communicate with the environment only through vertical eye movements and blinking which are the only motor outputs preserved. Despite this, consciousness and sensory pathways (exteroception, proprioception, vestibular inputs, and interoception) are completely conserved. Although cognition is also traditionally considered unaffected, due to the preservation of supratentorial structures, we recently described some non-motor symptoms in these patients, including motor imagery defects, selective emotional dysfunctions and pathological laughter and crying, and interpreted them as a consequence of a body representation disorder (Conson et al., 2008; Sacco et al., 2008; Pistoia et al., 2010). This fits with later volumetric data obtained in these patients, revealing the presence of an unexpected cortical loss involving areas typically associated with the mirror neuron system and the body matrix (Pistoia et al., 2016). As reminded by the authors, an accurate body representation is the result of the effective integration of multisensory (somatosensory, visual, auditory, vestibular, visceral) and motor signals, which provides an evolutionary advantage by maintaining a homeostatic protective milieu for human beings. This system, subserved by cortico-ponto-cerebellar pathways, matches bodily sensations and motor intentions in order to protect the body by triggering perceptual and behavioral programs (effectors) when something alters the body and the space around it (Riva et al., 2017). In patients with LIS, the lack of functioning efferent pathways, both at corticospinal and cortico-cerebellar level, may interfere with the body representation system, weaken the boundaries of the body and lead to unexpected symptoms in cognitive domains. Specifically patients are
Research Interests:
Background. Establishing prognosis in patients in a persistent vegetative state (VS) is still challenging. Neural networks underlying consciousness may be regarded as complex systems whose outputs show a degree of unpredictability... more
Background. Establishing prognosis in patients in a persistent vegetative state (VS) is still challenging. Neural networks underlying consciousness may be regarded as complex systems whose outputs show a degree of unpredictability experimentally quantifiable by means of nonlinear parameters such as approximate entropy (ApEn). Objective. The authors propose that the VS might be the result of derangement of the above neural networks, with an ensuing decrease in complexity and mutual interconnectivity: this might lead to a functional isolation within the cerebral cortex and to a reduction in the chaotic behavior of its outputs, with monotony taking the place of unpredictability. To test this hypothesis, the authors investigated whether nonlinear dynamics methods applied to electroencephalography (EEG) recordings may be able to predict outcomes. Methods. A total of 38 vegetative patients and 40 matched healthy controls were investigated. At admission, all patients were assessed by means of the Extended Glasgow Outcomes Coma Scale (EGOS) and the Coma Recovery Scale–Revised (CRS-R). At the same time an EEG recording was performed and used for time series analysis and ApEn computation. Patients were clinically reassessed at 6 months from the first evaluation. Results. Mean ApEn values (0.73, standard deviation [SD] = 0.12 vs 0.97, SD = 0.02; P < .001) were lower in patients than in controls. Patients with the lowest ApEn values either died (n = 14) or remained in a VS (n = 12), whereas patients with the highest ApEn values became minimally conscious (n = 5) or showed partial (n = 4) or full recovery (n = 3). Conclusions. These findings suggest that dynamic correlates of neural residual complexity might help in predicting outcomes in vegetative patients.
Consciousness has not yet been satisfactorily defined because of its 9 puzzling nature which involve the perception of the environment (perceptual 10 awareness) and of the self (self-awareness). Current available methods fail in 11... more
Consciousness has not yet been satisfactorily defined because of its 9 puzzling nature which involve the perception of the environment (perceptual 10 awareness) and of the self (self-awareness). Current available methods fail in 11 establishing prognosis in patients with vegetative state (VS): to our mind, this 12 failure stems from the heterogeneous localization of brain damages causing VS 13 and from available approaches tending to investigate self-awareness separately 14 from perceptual awareness, whereas consciousness should be explored as a 15 single and indivisible whole. Moving from the assumption that consciousness 16 depends on the normal activity of wide neural networks, that may be regarded 17 as complex systems whose outputs show a nonlinear behaviour, we propose a 18 nonlinear approach applied to electroencephalographic (EEG) signal, aimed at 19 exploring residual neural networks complexity in patients with VS. For this 20 objective the EEG recording of 10 patients previously admitted to our 21 department were retrospectively analyzed and compared with those of ten 22 matched healthy control subjects. Approximate Entropy (ApEn) was calculated 23 from the average values of time series with fixed input variables. Mean ApEn 24 values were lower in patients then in controls (t 18 =12.3, p < 0.001). ApEn is 25 able to discriminate patients from controls thus supporting the hypothesis about 26 a decreased neural networks complexity in VS. 27 Key Words: approximate entropy, vegetative state, nonlinear, complexity, coma 28 PREDICTING OUTCOMES IN PATIENTS WITH 29 DISORDERS OF CONSCIOUSNESS 30 What is most inscrutable for us human beings is human beings both 31 with regard to ourselves and our fellow creatures (Scriven, 1965). It is common 32 notion that the keystone to human unpredictable behaviour is consciousness and, 33 in particular, its puzzling nature with subjective and unconscious correlates. 34 Despite this, all attempts that throughout the ages have been made to satisfactory 35 define consciousness were unsuccessful because of its elusive essence that 36 cannot be localized within circumscribed brain areas as confirmed by the 37 1 Correspondence to: Marco Sarà,
Research Interests:
Patients diagnosed as vegetative have periods of wakefulness, but seem to be unaware of themselves or their environment. Although functional MRI (fMRI) studies have shown that some of these patients are consciously aware, issues of... more
Patients diagnosed as vegetative have periods of wakefulness, but seem to be unaware of themselves or their environment. Although functional MRI (fMRI) studies have shown that some of these patients are consciously aware, issues of expense and accessibility preclude the use of fMRI assessment in most of these individuals. We aimed to assess bedside detection of awareness with an electroencephalography (EEG) technique in patients in the vegetative state. This study was undertaken at two European centres. We recruited patients with traumatic brain injury and non-traumatic brain injury who met the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised definition of vegetative state. We developed a novel EEG task involving motor imagery to detect command-following--a universally accepted clinical indicator of awareness--in the absence of overt behaviour. Patients completed the task in which they were required to imagine movements of their right-hand and toes to command. We analysed the command-specific EEG responses of each patient for robust evidence of appropriate, consistent, and statistically reliable markers of motor imagery, similar to those noted in healthy, conscious controls. We assessed 16 patients diagnosed in the vegetative state, and 12 healthy controls. Three (19%) of 16 patients could repeatedly and reliably generate appropriate EEG responses to two distinct commands, despite being behaviourally entirely unresponsive (classification accuracy 61-78%). We noted no significant relation between patients&amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;#39; clinical histories (age, time since injury, cause, and behavioural score) and their ability to follow commands. When separated according to cause, two (20%) of the five traumatic and one (9%) of the 11 non-traumatic patients were able to successfully complete this task. Despite rigorous clinical assessment, many patients in the vegetative state are misdiagnosed. The EEG method that we developed is cheap, portable, widely available, and objective. It could allow the widespread use of this bedside technique for the rediagnosis of patients who behaviourally seem to be entirely vegetative, but who might have residual cognitive function and conscious awareness. Medical Research Council, James S McDonnell Foundation, Canada Excellence Research Chairs Program, European Commission, Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique, Mind Science Foundation, Belgian French-Speaking Community Concerted Research Action, University Hospital of Liège, University of Liège.
Research on the so-called " neural correlates of consciousness " (NCC) has come out of its niche, becoming a " trendy " matter both in scientific literature and mass media. It is a consequence of knowledge forthcoming about " vegetative... more
Research on the so-called " neural correlates of consciousness " (NCC) has come out of its niche, becoming a " trendy " matter both in scientific literature and mass media. It is a consequence of knowledge forthcoming about " vegetative state " (SV) and " minimally conscious state " (MCS). The idea of losing command of ourselves is indeed one of the greatest fears of this century. We are aware of the fact that we can find ourselves in a not-so-defined state (that may be definitive), which could be of more or less complete dependence on the others for an indefinite time. Thus, the reflection on the nature of consciousness, apart from representing one of the central scientific challenges of this new millennium , is also an urgent human necessity. While the problem of consciousness is no longer an exclusive object of philosophy, the discussion on the social consequences of these syndromes where there is impairment of consciousness has begun to raise ethical matters. Generally, all the scientific matters imply two kinds of fundamental questions: (1) those related to the nature of the problem itself; and (2) the possible solutions of this particular problem. Concerning the nature of consciousness, we face a " constitutional dilemma " for consciousness is not defined in a univocal way by various researchers over the world. What are the consequences of this heterogeneity? The most obvious deduction we can draw is that we have not defined yet what exactly is consciousness. This leads to inevitable consequences of scientific procedure: the lack of prerequisites of reproducibility. For example, " apples fall down, " and when Newton gave this phenomenon a certain mathematical description, he referred to something that everybody could observe in an absolutely comparable way. Thereby, if—for example—a Dutch or Australian scientist would have liked to verify the Newton equations, he would not have had problems doing it, confirming the laws of gravity. Unfortunately, with respect to consciousness, the situation is not the same given that a certain discovery made in a Japanese laboratory may not be confirmed by another laboratory where this problem (and also the expected answer) is defined in another way. Contemporary researchers assume consciousness as the condition in which we wake up in the morning after a no-dreams sleep (e.g., John Searle), while for many others, it is a combination of perception, synthesis, subjectivity, and free will. For some scientists (including the neurophilosopher Thomas Metzinger), consciousness itself has a lot of illusive elements, as well as free will (e.g., Benjamin Libet, Martin Heisenberg, and others). According to others, the only element that can be examined in a scientific way is the exact moment when a subject (obviously collaborative) confirms his or her awareness of a certain stimulus. To conclude, it is not easy to reproduce and compare the results achieved in different laboratories of the world: We put forth various questions, and thereby, the possible answers can be incomparable. In our opinion, dividing consciousness in parts implies " the paradox of the parts of an unknown whole " (Sarà & Pistoia, 2010), which leads us to wonder whether it is possible to study the distinct parts of a whole that we have not yet described in its entirety. Another important limit in studying patients with SV and MCS is represented by the considerable heterogeneity of brain damages that cause the above syndromes. However, we can affirm that the scientific literature is moving toward a " connectionist " view of cognitive functions and therefore of consciousness itself. The concept of functional integration is more and more spread in neuroscience. Particularly, the phenomenon of consciousness would emerge from dynamic shaping of neural functional aggregates even between distant cerebral areas. We have used the verb " emerge " to represent the types of phenomena (called " emergence phenomena ") among which, according to different authors, could be also consciousness. An emergent phenomenon is something that draws on what already exists in that space assuming its own identity. For example, a flock of birds (which is a dynamic aggregate of elements previously independent from each other), in aggregate, forms a new and clearly recognizable entity. We know that the aggregate will adopt its own behavior with advantages for itself and, above all, the elements that form it: single birds. The phenomena of emergence can be the subject of studies even when considering the laws of quantum mechanics with important advantages taking into account the matters related to nondeterministic behavior of so-called " voluntary " mental activity. An emergent phenomenon must necessarily be regarded in its entirety (also in line with the paradox of the parts of an unknown whole).
Research Interests:
In the present study, we demonstrated that observation of hand rotation had specific facilitation effects on a classical motor imagery task, the hand-laterality judgement. In Experiment 1, we found that action observation improved... more
In the present study, we demonstrated that observation of hand rotation had specific facilitation effects on a classical motor imagery task, the hand-laterality judgement. In Experiment 1, we found that action observation improved subjects’ performance on the hand laterality but not on the letter rotation task (stimulus specificity). In Experiment 2, we demonstrated that this facilitation was not due to mere observation of a moving hand, because it was triggered by observation of manual rotation but not of manual prehension movements (motion specificity). In Experiment 3, this stimulus- and motion-specific effect was found to be right hand-specific, compatible with left-hemispheric specialization in motor imagery but not in action observation. These data provided direct support to the idea that different simulation states, such as action observation and motor imagery, share some common mechanisms but also show specific functional differences.
Previous studies have been inconclusive whether dominant resting state alpha rhythms are greater or lower in amplitude in subjects with Down syndrome (DS) when compared to control subjects, ample resting alpha rhythms being considered as... more
Previous studies have been inconclusive whether dominant resting state alpha rhythms are greater or lower in amplitude in subjects with Down syndrome (DS) when compared to control subjects, ample resting alpha rhythms being considered as a reflection of good mechanisms of cortical neural synchronization. Here we tested the hypothesis that when the effects of head volume conduction are taken into account by the normalization of the cortical sources of resting alpha rhythms, these sources are lower in amplitude in DS subjects than in controls in line with typical findings in Alzheimer&amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;#39;s disease patients. Eyes-closed resting electroencephalographic (EEG) data were recorded in 45 DS subjects (25 males; mean age of 22.8years+/-0.7 standard error of mean (SEM)) and in 45 age-matched cognitively normal subjects (25 males; mean age of 22.4years+/-0.5 SEM). EEG rhythms of interest were delta (2-4Hz), theta (4-8Hz), alpha 1 (8-10.5Hz), alpha 2 (10.5-13Hz), beta 1 (13-20Hz), beta 2 (20-30Hz), and gamma (30-40Hz). Cortical EEG sources were estimated by low resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) and normalized across all voxels and frequencies. Central, parietal, occipital, and temporal cortical sources of resting alpha and beta rhythms were lower in amplitude in the DS than control subjects, whereas the opposite was true for occipital delta cortical sources. A control analysis on absolute source values showed that they were globally larger in amplitude across several frequency bands in DS than control subjects. These results suggest that normalized cortical sources of alpha rhythms are lower in amplitude in DS than control subjects, as it is typically found in Alzheimer&amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;#39;s disease. DS is accompanied by a functional impairment of cortical neuronal synchronization mechanisms in the resting state condition.
Previous studies have been inconclusive whether dominant resting state alpha rhythms are greater or lower in amplitude in subjects with Down syndrome (DS) when compared to control subjects, ample resting alpha rhythms being considered as... more
Previous studies have been inconclusive whether dominant resting state alpha rhythms are greater or lower in amplitude in subjects with Down syndrome (DS) when compared to control subjects, ample resting alpha rhythms being considered as a reflection of good mechanisms of cortical neural synchronization. Here we tested the hypothesis that when the effects of head volume conduction are taken into account by the normalization of the cortical sources of resting alpha rhythms, these sources are lower in amplitude in DS subjects than in controls in line with typical findings in Alzheimer&amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;#39;s disease patients. Eyes-closed resting electroencephalographic (EEG) data were recorded in 45 DS subjects (25 males; mean age of 22.8years+/-0.7 standard error of mean (SEM)) and in 45 age-matched cognitively normal subjects (25 males; mean age of 22.4years+/-0.5 SEM). EEG rhythms of interest were delta (2-4Hz), theta (4-8Hz), alpha 1 (8-10.5Hz), alpha 2 (10.5-13Hz), beta 1 (13-20Hz), beta 2 (20-30Hz), and gamma (30-40Hz). Cortical EEG sources were estimated by low resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) and normalized across all voxels and frequencies. Central, parietal, occipital, and temporal cortical sources of resting alpha and beta rhythms were lower in amplitude in the DS than control subjects, whereas the opposite was true for occipital delta cortical sources. A control analysis on absolute source values showed that they were globally larger in amplitude across several frequency bands in DS than control subjects. These results suggest that normalized cortical sources of alpha rhythms are lower in amplitude in DS than control subjects, as it is typically found in Alzheimer&amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;amp;#39;s disease. DS is accompanied by a functional impairment of cortical neuronal synchronization mechanisms in the resting state condition.

And 15 more