Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Skip to main content
Erich Round
  • School of Languages and Comparative Cultural Studies
    Gordon Greenwood Building (32)
    University of Queensland  QLD  4072
This book presents new data and a formal analysis of the inflectional system and syntax of Kayardild, a typologically striking language of Northern Australia. It sets forth arguments for recognizing an intricate syntactic structure that... more
This book presents new data and a formal analysis of the inflectional system and syntax of Kayardild, a typologically striking language of Northern Australia. It sets forth arguments for recognizing an intricate syntactic structure that underlies the exuberant distribution of inflectional features throughout the clause, and for an intermediate, 'morphomic' level of representation that mediates morphosyntactic features' realization as morphological forms.

The book differs from existing treatments of Kayardild in unifying the explanation of shared morphological exponents, positing a detailed, empirically-grounded underlying syntax, identifying new clausal and nominal structures, simplifying the analysis of Kayardild's dual tense system, rejecting an analysis according to which some case markers are morphologically 'verbalizing' and some tense markers 'nominalizing', and arguing that upper bounds on syntactic complexity are inherently syntactic rather than derivative of constraints on morphology.

Analyses are expressed formally in terms of syntactic structures and morphosyntactic features which will be interpretable to a broad range of theories. Early chapters provide overviews of Kayardild phonology and morphological structure in general, and a final chapter implements the analysis in constraint-based grammar. Example sentences are glossed across four or five lines, furnishing explicit analyses at multiple levels of representation, and an appendix gathers over one hundred examples sentences to provide large-scale empirical support for the syntactic analysis of tense inflection.
"Kayardild possesses one of, if not the, most exuberant systems of morphological concord known to linguists, and a phonological system which is intricately sensitive to its morphology. This dissertation provides a comprehensive... more
"Kayardild possesses one of, if not the, most exuberant systems of morphological concord known to linguists, and a phonological system which is intricately sensitive to its morphology. This dissertation provides a comprehensive description of the phonology of Kayardild, an investigation of its phonetics, its intonation, and a formal analysis of its inflectional morphology. A key component of the latter is the existence of a ‘morphomic’ level of representation intermediate between morphosyntactic features and underlying phonological forms.

Chapter 2 introduces the segmental inventory of Kayardild, the phonetic realisations of surface segments, and their phonotactics. Chapter 3 provides an introduction to the empirical facts of Kayardild word structure, outlining the kinds of morphs of which words are composed, their formal shapes and their combinations. Chapter 4 treats the segmental phonology of Kayardild. After a survey of the mappings between underlying and (lexical) surface forms, the primary topic is the interaction of the phonology with morphology, although major generalisations identifiable in the phonology itself are also identified and discussed. Chapter 5 examines Kayardild stress, and presents a constraint based analysis, before turning to an empirical and analytical discussion of intonation. Chapter 6, on the syntax and morphosyntax of Kayardild, is most substantial chapter of the dissertation. In association with the examination of a large corpus of new and newly collated data, mutually compatible analyses of the syntax and morphosyntactic features of Kayardild are built up and compared against less favourable alternatives. A critical review of Evans’ (1995a) analysis of similar phenomena is also provided. Chapter 7 turns to the realisational morphology — the component of the grammar which ties the morphosyntax to the phonology, by realising morphosyntactic features structures as morphomic representations, then morphomic representations as underlying phonological representations. A formalism is proposed in order to express these mappings within a constraint based grammar.

In addition to enriching our understanding of Kayardild, the dissertation presents data and analyses which will be of interest for theories of the interface between morphology on the one hand and phonology and syntax on the other, as well as for morphological and phonological theory more narrowly."
Based on recorded conversational data, this thesis describes the meanings of English some and certain, någon and viss, and offers some theoretical contributions following from those descriptions. Meaning is described within an... more
Based on recorded conversational data, this thesis describes the meanings of English some and certain, någon and viss, and offers some theoretical contributions following from those descriptions.

Meaning is described within an addressee-centred, (neo-) Gricean framework, with attention to relationships between total meanings (i.e., including implicature) as well as between clusters of bare, coded meanings.

At all times, meanings are related to the prosodic realisation of the tokens which carry them. Prosody is described within current autosegment-metrical models, to which minor contributions are made regarding Australian English and Götamål Swedish. Most notably, a system for the description of actual (non-abstract) rhythm is devised which proves fruitful in identifying additional prosodic cues to meaning beyond tone and segmental form.

The meanings investigated are as follows. Pure quantity meanings (i.e., ‘some’ versus ‘none’, ‘all’, ‘most’, ‘many’) are investigated in normal prosodic contexts and in contexts of ‘otherwise unjustifiably high prominence’, where extra implicatures are generated. These are analysed in a novel manner which nevertheless remains close to earlier proposals by Horn and Levinson in the field of semantics and Gussenhoven and Ladd in intonation. Subidentificational meanings ‘there was some guy...’ are related to particular prosodic configurations cued principally by (non-abstract) rhythm. The discriminative meanings of certain and viss are compared with specificity-based characterisations in the literature which are found to be overly restrictive. They are then considered alongside prosody and the lexical meanings of some and någon to account for why in English some and certain function as stylistic variants, while this is not true of Swedish någon and viss.

Outcomes of the study are as follows. Firstly, the autosegmental-metrical approach to prosody is applied successfully to spontaneous conversational data, with assistance from an augmented system for describing non-abstract rhythm. This rhythm is found to play an unexpectedly strong role in signalling meaning, and at the same time, this result calls into question the desirability of attempting to unify abstract and non-abstract rhythm: it is argued that these must be kept distinct. The segmental form of some is found to depend more on meaning and less on concurrent prosodic structure than proposed in some earlier accounts. Secondly, a Gricean model of meaning is found useful in describing meanings to a degree of both specificity and generality which captures language-internal and cross-language phenomena. Coupled with a view of meanings as meanings of signs, as opposed to ‘concepts’, a degree of explanation of the patterns observed is attained which, it is argued, would otherwise be absent.
Prehistoric human activities have contributed to the dispersal of many culturally important plants. The study of these traditional interactions can alter the way we perceive the natural distribution and dynamics of species and... more
Prehistoric human activities have contributed to the dispersal of many culturally important plants. The study of these traditional interactions can alter the way we perceive the natural distribution and dynamics of species and communities. Comprehensive research on native crops combining evolutionary and anthropological data is revealing how ancient human populations influenced their distribution. Although traditional diets also included a suite of non-cultivated plants that in some cases necessitated the development of culturally important technical advances such as the treatment of toxic seed, empirical evidence for their deliberate dispersal by prehistoric peoples remains limited. Here we integrate historic and biocultural research involving Aboriginal people, with chloroplast and nuclear genomic data to demonstrate Aboriginal-mediated dispersal of a non-cultivated rainforest tree. We assembled new anthropological evidence of use and deliberate dispersal of Castanospermum austral...
Phylogenetic comparative methods are new in our field and are shrouded, for most linguists, in at least a little mystery. Yet the path that led to their discovery in comparative biology is so similar to the methodological history of... more
Phylogenetic comparative methods are new in our field and are shrouded, for most linguists, in at least a little mystery. Yet the path that led to their discovery in comparative biology is so similar to the methodological history of balanced sampling, that it is only an accident of history that they were not discovered by a linguistic typologist. Here we clarify the essential logic behind phylogenetic comparative methods and their fundamental relatedness to a deep intellectual tradition focussed on sampling. Then we introduce concepts, methods and tools which will enable typologists to use these methods in everyday typological research. The key commonality of phylogenetic comparative methods and balanced sampling is that they attempt to deal with statistical non-independence due to genealogy. Whereas sampling can never achieve independence and requires most comparative data to be discarded, phylogenetic comparative methods achieve independence while retaining and using all comparati...
Almost universally, diachronic sound patterns of languages reveal evidence of both regular and irregular sound changes, yet an exception may be the languages of Australia. Here we discuss a long-observed and striking characteristic of... more
Almost universally, diachronic sound patterns of languages reveal evidence of both regular and irregular sound changes, yet an exception may be the languages of Australia. Here we discuss a long-observed and striking characteristic of diachronic sound patterns in Australian languages, namely the scarcity of evidence they present for regular sound change. Since the regularity assumption is fundamental to the comparative method, Australian languages pose an interesting challenge for linguistic theory. We examine the situation from two different angles. We identify potential explanations for the lack of evidence of regular sound change, reasoning from the nature of synchronic Australian phonologies; and we emphasise how this unusual characteristic of Australian languages may demand new methods of evaluating evidence for diachronic relatedness and new thinking about the nature of intergenerational transmission. We refer the reader also to Bowern (this volume) for additional viewpoints f...
Phylogenetic methods have broad potential in linguistics beyond tree inference. Here, we show how a phylogenetic approach opens the possibility of gaining historical insights from entirely new kinds of linguistic data – in this instance,... more
Phylogenetic methods have broad potential in linguistics beyond tree inference. Here, we show how a phylogenetic approach opens the possibility of gaining historical insights from entirely new kinds of linguistic data – in this instance, statistical phonotactics. We extract phonotactic data from 112 Pama-Nyungan vocabularies and apply tests for phylogenetic signal, quantifying the degree to which the data reflect phylogenetic history. We test three datasets: (1) binary variables recording the presence or absence of biphones (two-segment sequences) in a lexicon (2) frequencies of transitions between segments, and (3) frequencies of transitions between natural sound classes. Australian languages have been characterized as having a high degree of phonotactic homogeneity. Nevertheless, we detect phylogenetic signal in all datasets. Phylogenetic signal is greater in finer-grained frequency data than in binary data, and greatest in natural-class-based data. These results demonstrate the v...
Causal processes can give rise to distinctive distributions in the linguistic variables that they affect. Consequently, a secure understanding of a variable's distribution can hold a key to understanding the forces that have causally... more
Causal processes can give rise to distinctive distributions in the linguistic variables that they affect. Consequently, a secure understanding of a variable's distribution can hold a key to understanding the forces that have causally shaped it. A storied distribution in linguistics has been Zipf's law, a kind of power law. In the wake of a major debate in the sciences around power-law hypotheses and the unreliability of earlier methods of evaluating them, here we re-evaluate the distributions claimed to characterize phoneme frequencies. We infer the fit of power laws and three alternative distributions to 166 Australian languages, using a maximum likelihood framework. We find evidence supporting earlier results, but also nuancing them and increasing our understanding of them. Most notably, phonemic inventories appear to have a Zipfian-like frequency structure among their most-frequent members (though perhaps also a lognormal structure) but a geometric (or exponential) struct...
The chapter looks at language variation and change, and the correlation of these processes to language reconstruction and classification. The chapter gives an overview of theories, models, methods and data, describing how diversity and... more
The chapter looks at language variation and change, and the correlation of these processes to language reconstruction and classification. The chapter gives an overview of theories, models, methods and data, describing how diversity and variation is modelled and measured for reconstruction and classification within traditional comparative and statistical, evolutionary or phylogenetic methods. First, the chapter identifies the basic principles of language change and the way in which these differ within various subdomains of language. A second part delves around the outcome of change, describing the diverse result of sound change, lexical change, and typological/ morphosyntactic change. Here, important aspects include the inherent propensity of change, the role of arbitrariness, the role of systems, horizontal transfer, and the outcome of change at macro-levels. Finally, the chapter deals with the issue of the ontological status of the reconstruction, and how various theoretical approa...
In typology, rara provide valuable tests for theoretical hypotheses. Here I consider the rarum of PERSON inflection in Kayardild, which has only two surface contrasts but is found across all words in complementized subordinate clauses. I... more
In typology, rara provide valuable tests for theoretical hypotheses. Here I consider the rarum of PERSON inflection in Kayardild, which has only two surface contrasts but is found across all words in complementized subordinate clauses. I introduce a general schema for reasoning about the diachronic emergence of rara, and reconstruct the evolution of Kayardild subordinate PERSON agreement, from an earlier state in which a main‐clause inverse system was coupled to a system of complementizing CASE agreement. Serendipitously, the same synchronic facts have been analysed twice earlier without the benefit of the full diachronic backstory, and so present a retrospective case study in what diachrony offers for the analysis of rara, structures which by definition are difficult to contextualize using synchronic typology alone. I argue that since rara are so valuable for the testing of typological theories, and since diachrony may offer the only source of convincing explanation for them, it fo...
This article investigates the evolutionary and spatial dynamics of typological characters in 117 Indo-European languages. We partition types of change (i.e., gain or loss) for each variant according to whether they bring about a... more
This article investigates the evolutionary and spatial dynamics of typological characters in 117 Indo-European languages. We partition types of change (i.e., gain or loss) for each variant according to whether they bring about a simplification in morphosyntactic patterns that must be learned, whether they are neutral (i.e., neither simplifying nor introducing complexity) or whether they introduce a more complex pattern. We find that changes which introduce complexity show significantly less areal signal (according to a metric we devise) than changes which simplify and neutral changes, but we find no significant differences between the latter two groups. This result is compatible with a scenario where certain types of parallel change are more likely to be mediated by advergence and contact between proximate speech communities, while other developments are due purely to drift and are largely independent of intercultural contact.
Feature stability, time and tempo of change, and the role of genealogy versus areality in creating linguistic diversity are important issues in current computational research on linguistic typology. This paper presents a database... more
Feature stability, time and tempo of change, and the role of genealogy versus areality in creating linguistic diversity are important issues in current computational research on linguistic typology. This paper presents a database initiative, DiACL Typology, which aims to provide a resource for addressing these questions with specific of the extended Indo-European language area of Eurasia, the region with the best documented linguistic history. The database is pre-prepared for statistical and phylogenetic analyses and contains both linguistic typological data from languages spanning over four millennia, and linguistic metadata concerning geographic location, time period, and reliability of sources. The typological data has been organized according to a hierarchical model of increasing granularity in order to create datasets that are complete and representative.
Kala Lagaw Ya is the language of the western and central islands of the Torres Strait. It exhibits an extremely complex pattern of ‘split argument coding’ (‘split ergativity’), which has previously been considered typologically... more
Kala Lagaw Ya is the language of the western and central islands of the Torres Strait. It exhibits an extremely complex pattern of ‘split argument coding’ (‘split ergativity’), which has previously been considered typologically exceptional and problematic for widely discussed universals of argument coding dating back to work by Silverstein, Comrie and Dixon in the 1970s, and framed in terms of an ‘animacy’ or ‘nominal’ hierarchy. Furthermore, the two main dialects of the language, which centre around Saibai Island and Mabuiag Island, differ in the detail of their argument coding in interesting ways. In this paper we argue that once we take into account other typologically well-attested principles concerning the effect of markedness on neutralization in the morphological coding of grammatical categories, and in particular recent proposals about the typology of number marking systems, the Kala Lagaw Ya system falls into place as resulting from the unexceptional interaction of a number of universal tendencies. On this view, the case systems of the two dialects of Kala Lagaw Ya, while complex, appear not to be typologically exceptional. This account can be taken as a case study contributing to our understanding of universals of argument coding and how they relate to forces affecting the neutralization of morphological marking. The reframing of the Kala Lagaw Ya facts then has broader implications: it reinforces the value of viewing complex patterns as the result of the interaction of simpler, more regular forces, and in so doing it also lends further empirical weight to the universals of argument coding which Kala Lagaw Ya was previously thought to violate.
Modern, large-scale typology, with its enormous datasets and batteries of algorithms rather than humans doing the comparisons, is more sensitive than ever to choices in how to code up language data. Thus we see increasing theoretical... more
Modern, large-scale typology, with its enormous datasets and batteries of algorithms rather than humans doing the comparisons, is more sensitive than ever to choices in how to code up language data. Thus we see increasing theoretical emphasis on the need for variables which robustly compare like with like (Haspelmath 2010, et seq.); which typologize language facts, not quirks of descriptive traditions (Hyman 2014); which decompose traditional variables into their finer-grained constituent notions (Bickel 2010, Corbett 2005, et seq.); and which attend closely to the logical relationships between those constituents (Round 2013). But how does this theory translate into the nitty-gritty work of actually building such variables? We offer a view from the coalface, as a complement to these more theoretical lines of thought, as we attempt to modernize, decompose and scrutinize one traditional typological variable: the presence or absence in a language of phonemic pre-nasalised stops. Our fi...
Research Interests:
Debate over whether phonaesthemes are part of morphology has been long and inconclusive. We contend that this is because the properties that characterise individual phonaesthemes and those that characterise individual morphological units... more
Debate over whether phonaesthemes are part of morphology has been long and inconclusive. We contend that this is because the properties that characterise individual phonaesthemes and those that characterise individual morphological units are neither sufficiently disjunct nor sufficiently overlapping to furnish a clear answer, unless resort is made to relatively aprioristic exclusions from the set of ‘relevant’ data, in which case the answers follow directly and uninterestingly from initial assumptions. In response, we pose the question: ‘According to what criteria, if any, do phonaesthemes distinguish themselves from non-phonaesthemic, stem-building elements?’, and apply the methods of Canonical Typology to seek answers. Surveying the literature, we formulate seven canonical criteria, identifying individual phonaesthemes which are more, or less, canonical according to each. We next apply the same criteria to assess non-phonaesthemic stem-building elements. The result is that just one criterion emerges which clearly differentiates the two sets of phenomena, namely the canonical accompaniment of phonaesthemes by non-recurrent residues, and this finding is not predetermined by our assumptions. From the viewpoint of morphological theory more broadly, we assume that any viable theory must find a place for lexical stems which are composed of a recurring, sound-meaning pairing plus a non-recurrent residue. Most phonaesthemes will occur in such stems. Consequently, theoretically interesting questions can then be asked about this entire class of lexical stems, including but not limited to its phonaesthemic members. Whether they are ‘part of morphology’ or not, phonaesthemes can contribute coherently to the development of morphological theory.
Linguistics, and typology in particular, can have a bright future. We justify this optimism by discussing comparability from two angles. First, we take the opportunity presented by this special issue ofLinguistic Typologyto pause for a... more
Linguistics, and typology in particular, can have a bright future. We justify this optimism by discussing comparability from two angles. First, we take the opportunity presented by this special issue ofLinguistic Typologyto pause for a moment and make explicit some of the logical underpinnings of typological sciences, linguistics included, which we believe are worth reminding ourselves of. Second, we give a brief illustration of comparison, and particularly measurement, within modern typology.
Linguists seek insight from all human languages, however accessing information from most of the full store of extant global linguistic descriptions is not easy. One of the most common kinds of information that linguists have documented is... more
Linguists seek insight from all human languages, however accessing information from most of the full store of extant global linguistic descriptions is not easy. One of the most common kinds of information that linguists have documented is vernacular sentences, as recorded in descriptive grammars. Typically these sentences are formatted as interlinear glossed text (IGT). Most descriptive grammars, however, exist only as hardcopy or scanned pdf documents. Consequently, parsing IGTs in scanned grammars is a priority, in order to significantly increase the volume of documented linguistic information that is readily accessible. Here we demonstrate fundamental viability for a technology that can assist in making a large number of linguistic data sources machine readable: the automated identification and parsing of interlinear glossed text from scanned page images. For example, we attain high median precision and recall (>0.95) in the identification of examples sentences in IGT format. ...
Full text: https://doi.org/10.1075/dia.20004.mac Phylogenetic methods have broad potential in linguistics beyond tree inference. Here, we show how a phylogenetic approach opens the possibility of gaining historical insights from entirely... more
Full text: https://doi.org/10.1075/dia.20004.mac

Phylogenetic methods have broad potential in linguistics beyond tree inference. Here, we show how a phylogenetic approach opens the possibility of gaining historical insights from entirely new kinds of linguistic data-in this instance, statistical phonotactics. We extract phonotactic data from Pama-Nyungan vocabularies and apply tests for phylogenetic signal, quantifying the degree to which the data re ect phylogenetic history. We test three datasets: (1) binary variables recording the presence or absence of biphones (two-segment sequences) in a lexicon (2) frequencies of transitions between segments, and (3) frequencies of transitions between natural sound classes. Australian languages have been characterized as having a high degree of phonotactic homogeneity. Nevertheless, we detect phylogenetic signal in all datasets. Phylogenetic signal is greater in finer-grained frequency data than in binary data, and greatest in natural-class-based data. These results demonstrate the viability of employing a new source of readily extractable data in historical and comparative linguistics.
Autosegmental–metrical analyses of intonation typically assume a binary opposition between L/H tones, realised as pitch targets within some local pitch range, or register. However, because tone and register can be phonologically... more
Autosegmental–metrical analyses of intonation typically assume a binary opposition between L/H tones, realised as pitch targets within some local pitch range, or register. However, because tone and register can be phonologically independent, a theoretical concern is that an ostensibly three-leveled tone system could be analysed in terms of binary tone plus careful register setting. Plateau contours in Kayardild, based superficially around three tone levels, present a case in point. Arguments are provided that just two phonological tones are involved, plus a form of register control that characterises the entire Kayardild intonational system.
Research Interests:
Causal processes can give rise to distinctive distributions in the linguistic variables that they affect. Consequently, a secure understanding of a variable's distribution can hold a key to understanding the forces that have causally... more
Causal processes can give rise to distinctive distributions in the linguistic variables that they affect. Consequently, a secure understanding of a variable's distribution can hold a key to understanding the forces that have causally shaped it. A storied distribution in linguistics has been Zipf's law, a kind of power law. In the wake of a major debate in the sciences around power-law hypotheses and the unreliability of earlier methods of evaluating them, here we re-evaluate the distributions claimed to characterize phoneme frequencies. We infer the fit of power laws and three alternative distributions to 166 Australian languages, using a maximum likelihood framework. We find evidence supporting earlier results, but also nuancing them and increasing our understanding of them. Most notably, phonemic inventories appear to have a Zipfian-like frequency structure among their most-frequent members (though perhaps also a lognormal structure) but a geometric (or exponential) structure among the least-frequent. We compare these new insights the kinds of causal processes that affect the evolution of phonemic inventories over time, and identify a potential account for why, despite there being an important role for phonetic substance in phonemic change, we could still expect inventories with highly diverse phonetic content to share similar distributions of phoneme frequencies. We conclude with priorities for future work in this promising program of research.
Linguistics, and typology in particular, can have a bright future. We justify this optimism by discussing comparability from two angles. First, we make explicit some of the logical underpinnings of typological sciences, linguistics... more
Linguistics, and typology in particular, can have a bright future. We justify this optimism by discussing comparability from two angles. First, we make explicit some of the logical underpinnings of typological sciences, linguistics included, which we believe are worth reminding ourselves of. Second, we give a brief illustration of comparison, and particularly measurement, within modern typology.
In typology, rara provide valuable tests for theoretical hypotheses. Here I consider the rarum of PERSON inflection in Kayardild, which has only two surface contrasts but is found across all words in complementized subordinate clauses. I... more
In typology, rara provide valuable tests for theoretical hypotheses. Here I consider the rarum of PERSON inflection in Kayardild, which has only two surface contrasts but is found across all words in complementized subordinate clauses. I introduce a general schema for reasoning about the diachronic emergence of rara, and reconstruct the evolution of Kayardild subordinate PERSON agreement, from an earlier state in which a main-clause inverse system was coupled to a system of complementizing CASE agreement. Serendipitously, the same synchronic facts have been analysed twice earlier without the benefit of the full diachronic backstory, and so present a retrospective case study in what diachrony offers for the analysis of rara, structures which by definition are difficult to contextualize using synchronic typology alone. I argue that since rara are so valuable for the testing of typological theories, and since diachrony may offer the only source of convincing explanation for them, it follows that typological science will need to refer to diachrony for the successful development of theory. It cannot rely on synchrony alone.
Feature stability, time and tempo of change, and the role of genealogy versus areality in creating linguistic diversity are important issues in current computational research on linguistic typology. This paper presents a database... more
Feature stability, time and tempo of change, and the role of genealogy versus areality in creating linguistic diversity are important issues in current computational research on linguistic typology. This paper presents a database initiative, DiACL Typology, which aims to provide a resource for addressing these questions with specific of the extended Indo-European language area of Eurasia, the region with the best documented linguistic history. The database is pre-prepared for statistical and phylogenetic analyses and contains both linguistic typolog-ical data from languages spanning over four millennia, and linguistic metadata concerning geographic location, time period, and reliability of sources. The typological data has been organized according to a hierarchical model of increasing granularity in order to create data-sets that are complete and representative.
An automated method is presented for the commensurable, reproducible measurement of duration and lenition of segment types ranging from fully occluded stops to highly lenited variants, in acoustic data. The method is motivated with... more
An automated method is presented for the commensurable, reproducible measurement of duration and lenition of segment types ranging from fully occluded stops to highly lenited variants, in acoustic data. The method is motivated with respect to the relationship between acoustic and articulatory phonetics and, through subsequent evaluation, is argued to correspond well to articulation. It is then applied to the phonemic stops of casual speech in Gurindji (Pama-Nyungan, Australia) to investigate the nature of their articulatory targets. The degree of stop lenition is found to vary widely. Contrary to expectations, no evidence is found of a positive effect on lenition due to word-medial (relative to word-initial) position, beyond that attributable to duration; nor do non-coronals lenite more than their apical counterparts, which freely lenite along a continuum towards taps. No signi cant effect is found of preceding or following vocalic environment. Taken together, the observed lenition, duration, and peak intensity velocities are argued to be inconsistent with a single, fully-occluded articulatory ‘stop’ target which is undershot at short durations, rather targets can be understood to span a range or ‘window’ of values in the sense of Keating (1990), from fully-occluded stop-like targets to more approximant- like targets. It is an open question to what degree the patterns found in Gurindji are language particular, or can be related to the organization of obstruent systems in Australian languages more broadly. Precisely comparable studies of additional languages will be especially valuable in addressing these questions and others, and are possible using the method we introduce.
Anderson (2008) emphasizes that the space of possible grammars must be constrained by limits not only on what is cognitively representable, but on what is learnable. Focusing on word final deletion in Yidiny (Dixon 1977a), I show that the... more
Anderson (2008) emphasizes that the space of possible grammars must be constrained by limits not only on what is cognitively representable, but on what is learnable. Focusing on word final deletion in Yidiny (Dixon 1977a), I show that the learning of exceptional phonological patterns is improved if we assume that Prince & Tesar’s (2004) Biased Constraint Demotion (BCD) with Constraint Cloning (Pater 2009) is subject to a Morphological Coherence Principle (MCP), which operationalizes morphological analytic bias (Moreton 2008) during phonological learning. The existence of the MCP allows the initial state of CON to be simplified, and thus shifts explanatory weight away from the representation of the grammar per se, and towards the learning device.

I then argue that the theory of exceptionality must be phonological and diacritic. Specifically, I show that co-indexation between lexical forms and lexically indexed constraints must be via indices not on morphs but on individual phonological elements. Relative to indices on phonological elements, indices on morphs add computational cost for no benefit during constraint evaluation and learning; and a theory without indices on phonological elements is empirically insufficient. On the other hand, approaches which represent exceptionality by purely phonological means (e.g. Zoll 1996) are ill-suited to efficient learning. Concerns that a phonologically-indexed analysis would overgenerate (Gouskova 2012) are unfounded under realistic assumptions about the learner.
Features are central to all major theories of syntax and morphology. Yet it can be a non-trivial task to determine the inventory of features and their values for a given language, and in particular to determine whether to postulate one... more
Features are central to all major theories of syntax and morphology. Yet it can be a non-trivial task to determine the inventory of features and their values for a given language, and in particular to determine whether to postulate one feature or two in the same semantico-syntactic domain. We illustrate this from tense-aspect-mood (TAM) in Kayardild, and adduce principles for deciding in general between one-feature and two-feature analyses, thereby contributing to the theory of feature systems and their typology. Kayardild shows striking inflectional complexities, investigated in two major studies (Evans 1995a; Round 2013), and it proves particularly revealing for our topic. Both Evans and Round claimed that clauses in Kayardild have not one but two concurrent TAM features. While it is perfectly possible for a language to have two features of the same type, it is unusual. Accordingly, we establish general arguments which would justify postulating two features rather than one; we then apply these specifically to Kayardild TAM. Our finding is at variance with both Evans and Round; on all counts, the evidence which would motivate an analysis in terms of one TAM feature or two is either approximately balanced, or clearly favours an analysis with just one. Thus even when faced with highly complex language facts, we can apply a principled approach to the question of whether we are dealing with one feature or two, and this is encouraging for the many of us seeking a rigorous science of typology. We also find that Kayardild, which in many ways is excitingly exotic, is in this one corner of its grammar quite ordinary.
Typological datasets for quantitative historical-linguistic inquiry are growing in breadth, but a challenge is also to increase their depth, since advanced methods often ideally require many hundreds of traits per language. Using biphone... more
Typological datasets for quantitative historical-linguistic inquiry are growing in breadth, but a challenge is also to increase their depth, since advanced methods often ideally require many hundreds of traits per language. Using biphone transition probabilities from phonemicized vocabulary data, we extract several hundred high-definition phonotactic traits per language, for 17 languages in the Ngumpin-Yapa and Yolngu subgroups of the Pama-Nyungan family, Australia. We detect phylogenetic signal at a significant level (p < 0.001 for both subgroups), measured against a reference phylogeny inferred from basic vocabulary cognacy data. This contrasts with simpler, binary coding of biphones’ occurrence, which provides insufficient detail for the detection of phylogenetic signal. Thus, we demonstrate the viability of a new method in quantitative historical linguistics, and emphasize the inferential power to be harnessed from high-definition, trait-rich datasets for comparative research.
Research Interests:
Morphomes (Aronoff 1994) exemplify extreme complexity within morphology. This chapter argues that morphomic categories come in three types. Rhizomorphomes pertain to morphological roots, dividing the lexicon into classes (e.g.... more
Morphomes (Aronoff 1994) exemplify extreme complexity within morphology. This chapter argues that morphomic categories come in three types. Rhizomorphomes pertain to morphological roots, dividing the lexicon into classes (e.g. declensions, conjugation classes) whose members share similar paradigms. Meromorphomes pertain sets of word formation operations, which derive the pieces of individual word forms, thus meromorphomes inhere in the organisation of a morphological exponence system. Metamorphomes pertain to distributions of meromorphomes across a paradigm. Rhizomorphomes and metamorphomes are well described, but meromorphomes much less so. Arguments are presented for the existence of meromorphomes, drawing on  evidence from Kayardild (Round 2013). It is observed that in given languages, all three kinds of morphomic category may divide into subcategories, adding more complexity to the picture. Nevertheless, the architecture of this complexity, into an autonomous layer of representation with subcategories, is familiar and qualitatively similar to other domains of grammar.
The contact history of the languages of the Eastern and Western Torres Strait has been claimed (e.g. by Dixon 2002, Wurm 1972, and others) to have been sufficiently intense as to obscure the genetic relationship of the Western Torres... more
The contact history of the languages of the Eastern and Western Torres Strait has been claimed (e.g. by Dixon 2002, Wurm 1972, and others) to have been sufficiently intense as to obscure the genetic relationship of the Western Torres Strait language. Some have argued that it is an Australian (Pama-Nyungan) language, though with considerable infl uence from the Papuan language Meryam Mir (the Eastern Torres Strait language). Others have claimed that the Western Torres language is, in fact, a genetically Papuan language, though with substantial Australian substrate or adstrate influence. Much has been made of phonological structures which have been viewed as unusual for Australian languages. In this paper we examine the evidence for contact claims in the region. We review aspects of the phonology, morphology, syntax and lexicon of the Eastern and Western Torres Strait languages with an eye to identifying areal infl uence. Th is larger data pool shows that the case for intense contact has been vastly overstated. Beyond some phonological features and some loan words, there is no linguistic evidence for intense contact; moreover, the phonological features adduced to be evidence of contact are also found to be not specifically Papuan, but part of a wider set of features in Australian languages.
The non-Pama-Nyugan, Tangkic languages were spoken until recently in the southern Gulf of Carpentaria, Australia. The most extensively documented are Lardil, Kayardild and Yukulta. Their phonology is notable for its opaque, word-final... more
The non-Pama-Nyugan, Tangkic languages were spoken until recently in the southern Gulf of Carpentaria, Australia. The most extensively documented are Lardil, Kayardild and Yukulta. Their phonology is notable for its opaque, word-final deletion rules and extensive word-internal sandhi processes. The morphology contains complex relationships between sets of forms and sets of functions, due in part to major historical refunctionalizations, which have converted case markers into markers of tense and complementization, and verbal suffixes into case markers. Syntactic constituency is often marked by inflectional concord, resulting frequently in affix stacking. Yukulta in particular possesses a rich set of inflection-marking possibilities for core arguments, including detransitivised configurations and an inverse system. These relate in interesting ways historically to argument marking in Lardil and Kayardild. Subordinate clauses are marked for tense across most constituents other than the subject, and such tense marking is also found in main clauses in Lardil and Kayardild, which have lost the agreement and tense-marking second-position clitic of Yukulta. Under specific conditions of co-reference between matrix and subordinate arguments, and under certain discourse conditions, clauses may be marked, on all or almost all words, by complementization markers, in addition to inflection for case and tense.
A revised model of Tangkic linguistic and cultural history is developed based on a reanalysis of relationships between six Tangkic languages in the southern Gulf of Carpentaria and drawing on recent archaeological and environmental... more
A revised model of Tangkic linguistic and cultural history is developed based on a reanalysis of relationships between six Tangkic languages in the southern Gulf of Carpentaria and drawing on recent archaeological and environmental studies. Bayesian phylogenetic analysis of Tangkic basic vocabulary was employed to infer the topology of the Tangkic family tree and define structural branching events. Contrary to previous models suggesting progressive colonisation and fissioning from mainland sources, the data support hypotheses that the modern configuration of Tangkic owes its form to pulses of outward movement from Mornington Island followed by subsequent linguistic divergence in both grammar and lexicon of the varieties. We also speculate that an extreme environmental event (c.800-400 BP) may have flooded low-lying coastal areas resulting in abandonment of some areas, a relatively short co-residence involving cultural and linguistic syncretism between neighbouring groups and then recolonization.
The word final phonology of Lardil was brought to the attention of linguists by Ken Hale in the 1960s and since then certain properties of the data have led it to occupy a privileged position, in a canon of data sets against which new... more
The word final phonology of Lardil was brought to the attention of linguists by Ken Hale in the 1960s and since then certain properties of the data have led it to occupy a privileged position, in a canon of data sets against which new theoretical proposals are frequently tested. Several seminal arguments for new and high-profile phonological theories are now based at least in part upon analyses of Hale’s data set. After reviewing what is of such interest in Lardil, a body of data is assembled which alters our understanding of the empirical facts and theoretical implications of Lardil phonology. Hale’s process of Laminalization is reanalyzed as Apicalization; constrained lexical exceptions are found with respect to Apocope, Apicalization and Truncation; and a process of Raising is identified. A discussion of the systematicity of these new data, and of their demonstrable antiquity leads to the conclusion that future formal analyses of the language must account not only for already well-known properties of the data, but for the existence of multiple, active patterns that apply selectively throughout the lexicon.
Pre-stopped nasals and laterals occur in several Australian languages, but why? Nasal pre-stopping likely enhances cues to place of articulation contrasts (Butcher 1999, 2006). Though recent work proposes that lateral pre-stopping does... more
Pre-stopped nasals and laterals occur in several Australian languages, but why? Nasal pre-stopping likely enhances cues to place of articulation contrasts (Butcher 1999, 2006). Though recent work proposes that lateral pre-stopping does likewise (Keyser & Stevens 2006:fn.7; Loakes et al. 2008), the supporting argumentation appears unconvincing upon scrutiny. With reference to enhancement theory, articulatory phonology, and articulatory and acoustic phonetics, I speculate on: plausible diachronic sources of pre-stopped nasals via stochastic and functional processes; the extension of pre-stopping to laterals through a drive for regularity in the organisation of gesturally complex segments (Browman & Goldstein 1995; inter alia); and explanations for the asymmetrical prevalence of pre-stopping across places of articulation (Hercus 1972). Both language use and innate universals are crucial.
Debate over whether phonaesthemes are part of morphology has been long and inconclusive. We contend that this is because the properties that characterise individual phonaesthemes and those that characterise individual morphological units... more
Debate over whether phonaesthemes are part of morphology has been long and inconclusive. We contend that this is because the properties that characterise individual phonaesthemes and those that characterise individual morphological units are neither sufficiently disjunct nor sufficiently overlapping to furnish a clear answer, unless resort is made to relatively aprioristic exclusions from the set of ‘relevant’ data, in which case the answers follow directly and uninterestingly from initial assumptions. In response, we pose the question: ‘According to what criteria, if any, do phonaesthemes distinguish themselves from non-phonaesthemic, stem-building elements?’, and apply the methods of Canonical Typology to seek answers. Surveying the literature, we formulate seven canonical criteria, identifying individual phonaesthemes which are more, or less, canonical according to each. We next apply the same criteria to assess non-phonaesthemic stem-building elements. The result is that just one criterion emerges which clearly differentiates the two sets of phenomena, namely the canonical accompaniment of phonaesthemes by non-recurrent residues, and this finding is not predetermined by our assumptions. From the viewpoint of morphological theory more broadly, we assume that any viable theory must find a place for lexical stems which are composed of a recurring, sound-meaning pairing plus a non-recurrent residue. Most phonaesthemes will occur in such stems. Consequently, theoretically interesting questions can then be asked about this entire class of lexical stems, including but not limited to its phonaesthemic members. Whether they are ‘part of morphology’ or not, phonaesthemes can contribute coherently to the development of morphological theory.
Kala Lagaw Ya is the language of the Western and Central islands of the Torres Strait. It exhibits an extremely complex pattern of ‘split argument coding’ (‘split ergativity’), which has previously been considered typologically... more
Kala Lagaw Ya is the language of the Western and Central islands of the Torres Strait. It exhibits an extremely complex pattern of ‘split argument coding’ (‘split ergativity’), which has previously been considered typologically exceptional, and problematic for widely discussed universals of argument coding dating back to work by Silverstein, Comrie and Dixon in the 1970s, and framed in terms of an ‘animacy’ or ‘nominal’ hierarchy. Furthermore, the two main dialects of the language, which centre around Saibai Island and Mabuiag Island, differ in the detail of their argument coding in interesting ways. In this paper we argue that once we take into account other typologically well-attested principles concerning the effect of markedness on neutralization in the morphological coding of grammatical categories, and in particular recent proposals about the typology of number marking systems, the Kala Lagaw Ya system falls into place as resulting from the unexceptional interaction of a number of universal tendencies.  On this view, the case systems of the two dialects of Kala Lagaw Ya, while complex, appear not to be typologically exceptional. This account can be taken as a case study contributing to our understanding of universals of argument coding and how they relate to forces affecting the neutralization of morphological marking. The reframing of the Kala Lagaw Ya facts then has broader implications: it reinforces the value of viewing complex patterns as the result of the interaction of simpler, more regular forces, and in so doing it also lends further empirical weight to the universals of argument coding which Kala Lagaw Ya was previously thought to violate.
What is the full range of ways in which morphomes can figure in the organization of a morphological system? If we believe that arguments for the existence of morphomes are compelling, then this question demands attention. It is argued... more
What is the full range of ways in which morphomes can figure in the organization of a morphological system? If we believe that arguments for the existence of morphomes are compelling, then this question demands attention. It is argued that morphomic organization can extend even to morphotactics. Earlier research (Round 2013, 2015) establishes that the realisation of morphosyntactic feature-values in Kayardild requires a morphomic analysis if empirical generalisations are to be adequately expressed within a formal account. Here it is demonstrated that morphotactic constraints, which determine licit and illicit morphological strings in Kayardild, operate in terms of the same morphomic categories motivated by other aspects of the inflectional system. Consequences and priorities for future morphomic research are discussed.
The notion of ‘erosion’, a universal diachronic process affecting the phonetic content of certain language forms, has held a place in historical linguistics for almost two centuries now. Recently it has been argued that the erosion of... more
The notion of ‘erosion’, a universal diachronic process affecting the phonetic content of certain language forms, has held a place in historical linguistics for almost two centuries now. Recently it has been argued that the erosion of high frequency words can be derived as a consequence of normal language use within a theory of phonology based on exemplars. Focusing on discrete changes to function words, this paper argues that types of erosion exist which cannot be derived in this manner. Instead, erosion as well as other less celebrated, but well attested, irregular changes to function words can be accounted for by a species of paradigm levelling. Prosodic paradigm levelling (PPL) is much like its familiar morphological cousin only it plays out over paradigms whose cells contain word forms selected for by prosodic, not morphological, features. While PPL can account for data which exemplar models cannot, it is maintained nevertheless that exemplar models can offer a reasonable account of much of the data, provided that the model incorporates a discrete level of phonological representation, in addition to exemplars. Arguments presented have implications for phonological representation in general, as well as for the explanation of discrete, irregular change to function words.
Swedish någon has a number of ‘subidentificational’ meanings familiar from English some (eg he’s looking for some book). These meanings of någon are examined and analysed within a (neo-)gricean paradigm, whereupon four coded,... more
Swedish någon has a number of ‘subidentificational’ meanings familiar from English some (eg he’s looking for some book). These meanings of någon are examined and analysed within a (neo-)gricean paradigm, whereupon four coded, subidentificational meanings are identified. The central coded, subidentificational meaning is argued to be the ‘undisclosed identity’ of the NP’s discourse referent, which, when complemented by normal processes of conversational inference, leads to meanings-in-context such as ‘lack of Speaker knowledge/ recall/ interest’. Other coded meanings are ‘hindered identification’ and two hedges. The paper concludes by addressing the question of why these meanings, if demonstrably distinct, should nevertheless be associated with the one form, någon. Arguments from usage lead to some non-trivial conclusions.
Kalaw Lagaw Ya (KLY) has been noted twice, by Comire and by Dixon, to be typologically irregular in its animacy-based system of split case coding (also called split ergativity). However, it can be argued that this is not the case provided... more
Kalaw Lagaw Ya (KLY) has been noted twice, by Comire and by Dixon, to be typologically irregular in its animacy-based system of split case coding (also called split ergativity). However, it can be argued that this is not the case provided one takes into account certain other typologically well attested phenomena in the coding of grammatical and morphological categories, as well as the details of the KLYnumber system. One the one hand, this makes KLY appear typologically normal, and on the other, it lends further empirical weight to the case coding universals which KLY was previously thought to violate.
This description was prepared for the Gangalidda Dictionary compiled by Cassy Nancarrow, published by the Carpentaria Land Council Aboriginal Corporation and launched in Bourketown on June 18, 2014. Although the text is aimed at a... more
This description was prepared for the Gangalidda Dictionary compiled by Cassy Nancarrow, published by the Carpentaria Land Council Aboriginal Corporation and launched in Bourketown on June 18, 2014.

Although the text is aimed at a non-technical audience, it may be of interest to the linguistic community insofar as it contains analysis, especially of the morphology and the clitic complex, which attempts to go beyond the treatment in Keen (1983). All examples are from my own transcriptions and analysis of Sandra Keen’s field recordings
Research Interests:
Inferential–realisational analyses formalise a language’s inflectional morphology in terms of a mapping from a lexical index and a representation of morphosyntactic properties to a phonological form. Round (2009) has argued that the... more
Inferential–realisational analyses formalise a language’s inflectional morphology in terms of a mapping from a lexical index and a representation of morphosyntactic properties to a phonological form. Round (2009) has argued that the Australian language Kayardild requires the postulation of an intermediate level of representation, identified with Aronoff’s (1994) notion of a ‘morphome’. This morphomic level serves to express patterns of identities of exponence abstracted away from the actual forms of exponents, and its use makes possible the expression of certain identities of form which defy expression by means of Rules of Referral (Zwicky 1985, Stump 1993). This paper considers identities of form that span the inflection–derivation divide in Kayardild and shows that they too are coherently captured by assuming that a morphomic level of representation is present. A consequence is that lexical stems must possess a morphomic representation in addition to their representations on other levels.
Prosodic prominence and constituency are frequently mani¬fested in the speech signal through segment duration. Short vowels in Kayardild (Tangkic) are examined for such correlates. Acoustic analysis of spontaneous monologues from two... more
Prosodic prominence and constituency are frequently mani¬fested in the speech signal through segment duration. Short vowels in Kayardild (Tangkic) are examined for such correlates. Acoustic analysis of spontaneous monologues from two speakers provides evidence for lengthening in utterance initial and final syllables. No compression of vowels is found in connection with larger foot size. Evidence indicates lengthening of stressed low vowels and high back vowels but not of high front vowels. There is evidence for lengthening of vowels in open syllables. Vulnerabilities for endangered language research reliant on spontaneous speech, and feasible methodological improvements, are discussed.
Autosegmental–metrical analyses of intonation typically assume a binary opposition between L/H tones, realised as pitch targets within some local pitch range, or register. However, because tone and register can be phonologically... more
Autosegmental–metrical analyses of intonation typically assume a binary opposition between L/H tones, realised as pitch targets within some local pitch range, or register. However, because tone and register can be phonologically independent, a theoretical concern is that an ostensibly three leveled tone system could be analysed in terms of binary tone plus careful register setting. Plateau contours in Kayardild, based superficially around three tone levels, present a case in point. Arguments are provided that just two phonological tones are involved, plus a form of register control that characterises the entire Kayardild intonational system.
Contents: 18.5hrs video; 95.5hrs audio; 5,600 words of 5- and 7-line interlinearglossed text (vernacular, phonemic, morphomic, morphosyntactic, free translation; intonation & stress); 17,500 words of additional transcribed text; 255... more
Contents: 18.5hrs video; 95.5hrs audio; 5,600 words of 5- and 7-line interlinearglossed text (vernacular, phonemic, morphomic, morphosyntactic, free translation; intonation & stress); 17,500 words of additional transcribed text; 255 versus of song; full metadata.
Contents: 16hrs video; 60hrs audio; 8,600 words of 5-line interlinear-glossed text; 3,700 words of additional transcribed text; full metadata.
This chapter introduces a method for deriving historical syntactic hypotheses from certain types of phonological reconstructions. The method, edge aligned reconstruction, capitalises on the robust typological generalisation that the edges... more
This chapter introduces a method for deriving historical syntactic hypotheses from certain types of phonological reconstructions. The method, edge aligned reconstruction, capitalises on the robust typological generalisation that the edges of high level phonological domains, such as the utterance and intonation phrase, align almost always with the edges of major syntactic domains such as the sentence and the clause (Selkirk 1984, Nespor and Vogel 1986). Essentially, because the highest levels of phonological and syntactic domains are aligned with one another, if a phonological change is reconstructed as having occurred solely at the left edge, right edge, or in the interior of a high level phonological domain, then we predict that the change will have impacted differently on words in initial, final or in medial position within the corresponding syntactic domain. That is, if words can be identified which must have occupied certain phonological positions when the change occurred, then hypotheses can also be formulated regarding their syntax. Furthermore, because historical phonological facts are generally reconstructable back much further in time than is often the case with historical syntactic facts, this method should provide access to syntax at a time depth which is not usually accessible. To illustrate the use of this method, examples are discussed from the Tangkic language family of Queensland, Australia.

And 2 more

Citation: Macklin-Cordes, J. L. & Round, E. R. (2016). High-definition phonotactic data contain phylogenetic signal. Poster presented at the 90th Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America. Washington, D. C. See the extended... more
Citation:
Macklin-Cordes, J. L. & Round, E. R. (2016). High-definition phonotactic data contain phylogenetic signal. Poster presented at the 90th Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America. Washington, D. C.

See the extended paper:
Macklin-Cordes, J. L. & Round, E. R. (2015). High-definition phonotactics reflect linguistic pasts. Proceedings of the 6th Conference on Quantitative Investigations in Theoretical Linguistics. Tübingen: University of Tübingen. 5pp. http://dx.doi.org/10.15496/publikation-8609

Historical linguistic datasets are growing broader in terms of languages under study. However, a challenge is to also increase the depth of such datasets, since modern methods often ideally require hundreds of characters per language for statistical validity.
We extract many hundreds of high-definition characters from the phonotactics of two Australian language groups (Ngumpin-Yapa and Yolngu) and demonstrate that these contain phylogenetic signal. Thus, we demonstrate an important path towards power-intensive, modern methods.
Research Interests:
Advanced quantitative research is strengthened by large, high-quality data. As linguistics expands its stock of broad typological datasets, we review how well the data is suited to these new, non-manual methods of analysis. A key concern... more
Advanced quantitative research is strengthened by large, high-quality data. As linguistics expands its stock of broad typological datasets, we review how well the data is suited to these new, non-manual methods of analysis. A key concern is with ‘binning’, that is, the summarising of fine-grained information into coarser grained categories, and labelling of binned categories, which can be variable, ambiguous, biased and circular. We highlight solutions to these problems based on un-binning the data. This process is critical and meta-theoretical and thus not easy, but the payoff is high, as it leads to significantly larger, higher quality datasets and all that the benefits they bring.
Research Interests:
Since it was first described in extensive detail by R.M.W.Dixon (1977a,b), the phonology of Yidiny (Paman, Queensland Australia) has featured prominently in key debates within theoretical phonology. The linguistic system of Yidiny has... more
Since it was first described in extensive detail by R.M.W.Dixon (1977a,b), the phonology of Yidiny (Paman, Queensland Australia) has featured prominently in key debates within theoretical phonology. The linguistic system of Yidiny has much to keep phonologists busy, with complex interactions between stress, vowel length, morphotactics and word-final deletion, some of which are also subject to idiosyncratic morphological exceptions. The phenomena of stress and vowel length have received particularly intense attention within the development of modern theories of meter and prosody (Hayes 1982, 1985; Kager 1993; Crowhurst & Hewitt 1995; Hall 2001; Pruitt 2011; Kaviloda & Lunden 2014, inter alia). By contrast, the process of word final deletion has received relatively less attention, particularly in relation to its claimed conditioning by morpheme boundaries (Dixon 1977a:58). Given that our understanding of all three processes in Yidiny is interdependent, there may be benefits at a general level, to gaining increased clarity with respect to final deletion. With this in mind, we demonstrate that word final deletion in Yidiny is a simpler process than previously described.

A close examination of the complete morphological inventory of Yidiny shows that in Dixon’s (1977a,b) analysis, sensitivity to morpheme boundaries arises as a complex consequence of a single analytic decision as to which, out of two sets of just three suffixes, is regarded as exceptional. Reversing Dixon’s choice, from set #1 to set #2, permits us to recast the rule so that constraints on word-final phonotactics subsume the role Dixon had assigned to morpheme boundaries. Given that Dixon’s original rule also necessitated reference to constraints on word-final phonotactics, this means that our revision of the analysis represents a significant simplification, effectively folding two distinct conditioning factors into one. As confirmation that our reanalysis is on the right track, our revised account of word-final deletion also explains certain gaps in the Yidiny lexicon, which are accidental and indeed highly unexpected under Dixon’s analysis, but which are principled under ours. We implement our analysis in a constraint based grammar and show that it is simple, being expressible in terms of a small set of constraints pertaining to foot structure, word final phonotactics, and lexical exceptions.
Research Interests:
Modern, large-scale typology, with its enormous datasets and batteries of algorithms rather than humans doing the comparisons, is more sensitive than ever to choices in how to code up language data. Thus we see increasing theoretical... more
Modern, large-scale typology, with its enormous datasets and batteries of algorithms rather than humans doing the comparisons, is more sensitive than ever to choices in how to code up language data. Thus we see increasing theoretical emphasis on the need for variables which robustly compare like with like (Haspelmath 2010, et seq.); which typologize language facts, not quirks of descriptive traditions (Hyman 2014); which decompose traditional variables into their finer-grained constituent notions (Bickel 2010, Corbett 2005, et seq.); and which attend closely to the logical relationships between those constituents (Round 2013). But how does this theory translate into the nitty-gritty work of actually building such variables? We offer a view from the coalface, as a complement to these more theoretical lines of thought, as we attempt to modernize, decompose and scrutinize one traditional typological variable: the presence or absence in a language of phonemic pre-nasalised stops. Our findings help to assess the practicality of some recent, theoretically-motivated proposals, and to nuance the theory itself.
The presence or absence of pre-nasalised stops, as a typological variable, has figured in several recent, large-scale typological studies (Dunn et al. 2005, Reesink et al. 2009, Donohue et al. 2013), however preliminary investigation (Round 2013) suggested that the variable performed poorly at comparing like languages with like. In response, we set about to develop a finer-grained set of ‘micro-variables’, which should encode richer information and perform better at comparing like with like. We coded them for 280 Australian and Papua New Guinean doculects (i.e., descriptions of language varieties; Cysouw & Good 2013), and along the way paid particular attention to the challenges we encountered.
A first finding is that, as a process which researchers undertake, the decomposition of typo-logical variables is iterative. The aim of decomposing a macro-variable is to tease apart some of the linguistic properties which it conflates, and which would lead to false comparisons of unlike with unlike. Our experience shows that in reality, it is likely that after a given round of decomposition, there will be further conflations that emerge and require addressing. To take an example, after we had created separate micro-variables which interrogate the structure of consonant clusters in word initial/medial/final position, we noted that these still conflate languages in which certain clusters are rare and those in which they are common. Accordingly, it would be wrong to view the building of micro-variables as a ‘fell-swoop’ process, or one which replaces ‘imperfect variables’ with ‘perfect variables’. Rather, it is a process for improv¬ing dataset design in an iterative fashion.
A second finding is that the logical dependencies between variables, including those which can be problematic for the ‘big data’ statistics coming into currency, may also be iterative, or tree-like in nature. For example, we find that micro-variables focusing on consonant clusters in certain positions funnel logically into a disjunctive ‘meso-variable’ focused on clusters in general, which then feeds along with other micro-variables into the macro-variable ‘are there prenasalised stops’.
Finally, we find strong evidence that preliminary suspicions about our pre-nasalised stop macro-variable were well founded. The macro-variable ‘are there prenasalised stops’ is primarily, and covertly, a variable about the size of consonant clusters, but one which (i) does poorly at grouping like languages with like, and (ii) is modulated by a second micro-variable which appears to us to act as a proxy for different schools of linguistic analysis, and not linguistic facts. Therefore, we strongly endorse a rapid shift towards micro-variate typology (Bickel 2010; Round 2013) if our aim is to reach better, clearer and deeper generalizations about human languages and human Language, from sound empirical data.
Research Interests:
Australian languages are often cited for the unusual degree of homogeneity in their synchronic phonologies. In contrast, the fact that their diachronic sound patterns are equally if not more atypical has received little attention. We... more
Australian languages are often cited for the unusual degree of homogeneity in their synchronic phonologies. In contrast, the fact that their diachronic sound patterns are equally if not more atypical has received little attention. We delineate the nature of the problem posed by sound change in Australian languages, and identify promising directions for the elucidation, and explanation of this uncommon state of affairs. 

THE POVERTY OF AUSTRALIAN SOUND CHANGE  In the comparative method, demonstrations of cognacy play a central role. A convincing demonstration requires regular correspondences, of which a significant number involve non-identical sounds. Non-identical sounds are necessary, since correspondences that are merely identical could result not only from shared descent, but from heavy borrowing. Potential cognates in Australian languages display a degree of phonological similarity which, to our knowledge, is simply not encountered in language families in other parts of the world. Potential cognates are often near-identical, and furthermore there is little recurrence of correspondences, as the number of cognate sets is only around 200. Given its atypicality, is not surprising that current theory provides few answers as to what to do with such data. Yet while most historical linguists will never face the problem of having an overwhelming majority of their sound correspondences being near identical, in the Australian case it is an issue which demands some kind of response, and presents both a puzzle and a challenge for theories of sound change.

THE POVERTY OF AUSTRALIAN PHONOLOGICAL DIVERSITY  What, then, are the tools we have to work with?  Synchronically, Australian languages display uniformity in static properties of their phonological systems – phonemic inventories, phonotactic constraints, morpheme structure conditions, and metrical systems. A point worth noting however, is that these metrics ignore the question of dynamic (morphophonological) alternations. Since the synchronic morphophonological alternations in any language typically have sound change antecedents, one might hypothesize that on a continent of absent sound changes, morphophonology should likewise be impoverished. In fact though, this is not the case.

THE POVERTY OF DIVERSITY RECONSIDERED  Results emerging from Round’s large survey of Australian languages’ morphophonology may provide new insight into Australian phonological diachrony and synchrony. If synchronic deletion and lenition processes reflect diachrony in at least most cases, then one effect of Australian sound changes is a tendency to preserve the typical Australian phonemic inventory and phonotactic patterns. Additionally, these new findings may shed light on the lack of observed changes in Pama-Nyungan roots. Butcher (2006) argues that post-tonic consonants in Australian languages occupy prosodically ‘strong’ positions. Assuming that these are resistant to changes such as lenition and deletion, a consequence is that the typical disyllabic Pama-Nyungan root will not contain the most common targets of sound change. Further exploration of links of this nature strike us as promising.

THE ROLE OF MULTILINGUALISM  Another question worth considering is whether the paucity of sound change and apparent high rates of lexical replacement may, in part, be the result of normal transmission in a multilingual context. Results emerging from an experiment by Ellison and Miceli investigating lexical choices in code-switching bilinguals show a statistically significant bias towards the avoidance of word forms that are shared (cognates or borrowings), if alternative, distinct word forms are available in the target language.  Simulations show that, diachronically, this would result in a fast depletion of cognate word forms from related languages in sustained, multilingual contact.  This type of methodology, combining experimental observation with simulations, could also be extended to the study of phonological categories, potentially yielding further insights into the problem of Australian synchrony and diachrony.
The Pama-Nyungan language Yidiny has long held an important position in the typology of stress systems. Dixon’s (1977, 1990) original analysis of the system places alternating stress on odd-numbered syllables by default, as in (1a).... more
The Pama-Nyungan language Yidiny has long held an important position in the typology of stress systems. Dixon’s (1977, 1990) original analysis of the system places alternating stress on odd-numbered syllables by default, as in (1a). However, stress is attracted to long vowels (1b), which will cause other, alternating stresses to shift also (1c). Words with an odd number of syllables undergo penultimate lengthening, which in turn shifts stress onto even-numbered syllables (1d). Addition of a monsyllabic suffix (1e) changes the syllable count, with concomitant stress and length adjustments.

(1) a. yábulám-gu ‘lawyer cane-PURPOSIVE’
b. durgú: ‘mopoke owl(ABSOLUTIVE)’
c. yadyí:-ri-ŋá-l ‘walk about-GOING-TRANSITIVIZER-PRESENT’
d. gudá:ga ‘dog(ABSOLUTIVE)’
e. gúdagá-nggu ‘dog-ERGATIVE’

Additional complexities include suffixes which induce lengthening on their base and a late truncation rule, which is subject to lexical exceptions and applies after penultimate lengthening, rendering lengthening opaque. Accounting for these synchronic phenomena is Dixon’s main concern.

The system has proven a stubborn outlier within typologies of stress systems (Nash 1979, Hayes 1980, 1982, 1995, Halle and Vergnaud 1987, Crowhurst and Hewitt 1995, Pruitt 2011), however with the exception of Nash (1979), analyses of Yidiny stress have relied on the printed examples in Dixon’s works and taken the marking of length and stress as given. Here, we provide a new analysis of Yidiny stress, length, and truncation, based on observations from original recordings of the last fluent speakers.

Firstly, these recordings suggest a different analysis of Yidiny stress. We claim that Yidiny primary stress is always located on the first syllable of the word — it does not move to long vowels. We support this with acoustic analysis of recordings made by both Dixon and others of narrative and elicited data, which show the following characteristics:

• long vowels often have higher intensity than short, but not always;
• as in many Australian languages, feet associate with an L+H* pitch accent (Round 2009);
• the H* typically aligns within the first syllable, as a narrow or a broad peak (cf. Bowern et al 2012); this is true even in loan words from English (e.g. jígu:lgu ‘school-DAT’; Hale archive tape 4607);
• however, where a stressed syllable is followed by two unstressed syllables, its associated H* may align late, for example within the next syllable.

Significantly, for trisyllables with a long vowel in the second syllable, the phonetics of the long vowel often match the English cues for stress, as noted elsewhere for other Australian languages (Round 2009). Yet pitch is explained by the distance between stressed syllables, and intensity by vowel length. Therefore we find no need to claim that the long vowel is stressed, or that stress is optionally fronted (Dixon 1977:5), rather primary stress is always initial.

This has ramifications for Dixon’s (1977) analysis of the principles for length and stress assignment, and also for the many subsequent reinterpretations of Dixon’s data. In this paper, however, since we are arguing that the original observation of weight-to-stress is incorrect, we concentrate on the empirical arguments for initial stress; leave further discussion of the implications of this analysis to future work.
Secondly, although previous analyses (e.g. Hayes 1999, Dixon 1977) rely on Yidiny’s trisyllabic penultimate lengthening rule being automatic, we find exceptions to it, just as there are exceptions to truncation. For example, there is no expected penultimate lengthening in words such as jarruga ‘scrub hen’, dadagal ‘bone’ or duburrji ‘full up’ (Hale 4607). Conversely, we tentatively find what may be phrasal-level penultimate lengthening in some four-syllable words, and penultimate secondary stress on words with a long final vowel (e.g. gádigàdi: ‘little things’).

The diachronic sources of these facts are of crucial interest (cf. Hayes 1999). We account for the contemporary lengthening facts by a simple sound change involving penultimate lengthening and truncation, a type of compensatory lengthening well known from other languages (e.g. de Chene and Anderson 1979). Exceptions include loans from neighboring languages (particularly Djabugay). Postulation of a diachronic stress shift away from the first syllable is unnecessary. 

In conclusion, we show the value to phonological theory of revisiting claims made before the advent of easy access to acoustic data. It is now viable in many cases to conduct independent verification of analyses, based on original recordings.
Reduplication occurs in almost every human language, yet for languages with CV(C*) syllables, only a minority elect to reduplicate by infixing a VC* string. Reduplicants copy some or all of a base, where the base itself is some... more
Reduplication occurs in almost every human language, yet for languages with CV(C*) syllables, only a minority elect to reduplicate by infixing a VC* string.

Reduplicants copy some or all of a base, where the base itself is some contiguous string which sits to the left or right of the reduplicant. Reduplicants are often only partial copies, and often contain unmarked segments (e.g. short vowels) in place of base segments which are more marked (e.g. long vowels). Data like (1–3) raise the questions: (i) why does the reduplicant take on a VC* shape, and (ii) why is the reduplicant an infix within the word as a whole?
McCarthy & Prince (1993) analyse such reduplication as driven by the placement of the reduplicant within the word: it is attracted to the left edge, but a higher-ranking constraint denies it the absolute leftmost position. Consequently, the first segment of the reduplicant is the second segment of the word, and in order that syllables all retain a CV(C*) structure, the reduplicant will begin with a vowel. Pensalfini (1998) presents an alternative analysis, driven by shape: the reduplicant is attracted to the left edge but must begin with a vowel, and consequently, in order that all syllables have an onset, it becomes an infix. Also required for this analysis, is that reduplicants copy as many contiguous consonants as possible.

Intriguingly, Pensalfini’s account is driven by constraints which, if ranked high enough, would push a language to undergo initial consonant loss — a process which is historically attested in many Australian languages. Thus it would be enlightening to ascertain which analysis is ultimately correct: is VC* reduplication a placement-driven phenomenon, or is it a shape-driven process which contains the seeds of initial-dropping?

Kuuk Thaayorre (Paman, Gaby 2006) is situated on the south-west of Cape York peninsula, not far from many languages which have undergone initial dropping. Thaayorre itself has CV(C*) syllables. It also possesses infixing VC* reduplication for most verbal stem shapes (4–5), but not for stems containing a long first vowel, whose reduplication is CV (6–7). Significant here is that in Thaayorre, underlying vowel length is always preserved in the base. However, only the initial vowel of a Thaayorre word can be long, which means that a long vowel in the base cannot afford to be shunted to the right by an infix. Consequently, the infixing reduplicant starts at the third segment in the word. This fact allows us to contrast, and thus test, the placement-driven and shape-driven analyses. The former analysis predicts that the infix will stay as far to the left as possible, as in (7a); the latter predicts it will drift rightwards if by doing so it increases its number of copied consonants, as in (7b) [note: kt̪ in (7b) would be a perfectly legal cluster]. In fact, (7a) is the attested form.

Kuuk Thaayorre:
(4) REDUP; /ŋeɻnkan / → ŋ<eɻnk RED>[eɻnkan BASE]
(5) REDUP; /kal/ → k<al RED>[al BASE]
(6) REDUP; /koːpe/ →
a.  [koː BASE]<ko RED>pe
b.* k<oːp RED>[ope BASE]
(7) REDUP; /ti̪ ːk/ →
a. [ti̪ ː BASE]<ti̪ RED>k
b.*[ti̪ ːk BASE][ti̪ k RED]

This advances our understanding of the nature of reduplication: in a CV(C*) language, infixing VC* reduplication in the general case is driven not by shape, but by placement.
"Human languages can differ remarkably in the complexity of their morphophonology, and Australian languages are no exception. However, since the complexity of a linguistic analysis can also be due to the methods and assumptions of the... more
"Human languages can differ remarkably in the complexity of their morphophonology, and Australian languages are no exception. However, since the complexity of a linguistic analysis can also be due to the methods and assumptions of the analyst, when we are faced with a particularly complex analysis, it is informative for our understanding of both typology and linguistic practice to evaluate what complexity derives from the language itself, and what from the analyst's choices. We demonstrate the utility of this kind of enquiry by considering the morphophonology of Ngandi (Heath 1978).

Ngandi is an East Arnhem language, formerly spoken by at least five clans in the Rose River area, and one of five languages whose morphophonology was accorded a relatively complex analysis by Heath in a series of grammars written around 1980: Ngandi (1978); Ritharngu (1980); Warndarang (1980); Mara (1981) and Nunggubuyu (1984). Remaining within a rule-based paradigm, we demonstrate that some complexity is due to Heath's analytic choices and therefore can be reduced, though the language is undeniably rather complex itself. In particular, a more perspicuous analysis emerges when morphological conditioning is clearly partitioned from phonological conditioning. This is a practice which we strongly endorse when one's aim is to describe a language's morphophonology as perspicuously as possible. In addition, some complexity is reduced by reordering automatic, feature filling rules to a position late, rather than early, in the derivation, a principle which in various guises has been recognised by phonological theorists since pre-generative times.

Some specifics are as follows. Heath (1978) proposes (P-a) '[tense] feature filling', (Pb) 'morphologically conditioned hardening' and (P-c) 'lenition', which requires complex conditioning on (P-a) to derive a form like (1) where fG/ is underspecified for [tense].

(1) /yaŋ-Garu/ –(P-a)→ yaŋ-garu –(P-b)→ yaŋ-karu –(P-c)→ yaŋ-garu

By noticing that (P-c) serves to re-lenite those stops hardened by (P-b) in the same environment stated in the feature-filling rule (P-a), the analysis can be simplified by eliminating (P-c) and reformulating the 'feature filling rule' (P-a), now at the end of the derivation, as in (2):

(2) /yan-Garu/ –(P-b)→ yaŋ-karu –(P-a)→ yaŋ-garu

In addition, there is no need to characterise occlusive-final prefixes as conditioning hardening rule (P-b), since they are subsumed by the environment of (P-a) which always (re-)produces lenis morpheme-initial stops anyhow. The prefixes and stems which do trigger (P-b) are idiosyncratic and thus in any account of Ngandi, they will need to be specified lexically. Accordingly, following theories such as Lexical Phonology (Kiparsky (1982), Mohanan (1986)), we accord them a separate, morphologically determined stratum in which the hardening of (P-b) would apply; in other morphological contexts this stratum does not apply, and thus the final derivation can be simplified as in (3):

(3) /yan-Garu/ -(P-a)→ yaŋ-garu

Beyond the specifics of Ngandi, our results underscore the value in examining languages from multiple viewpoints. As with other, very well-studied languages, if our aim is to tease out the breadth and true nature of what is happening in the phonologies of human languages, then we will benefit from the availability of multiple analyses. By doing so, we can begin to clarify the impact of linguistic-analytic practice on our understanding of linguistic typology. "
Australian languages are known for their very low level of phonological diversity. Yet how and why just one continent should be so homogeneous is not understood. I report on results emerging from the first large scale study of Australian... more
Australian languages are known for their very low level of phonological diversity. Yet how and why just one continent should be so homogeneous is not understood. I report on results emerging from the first large scale study of Australian morphophonemics, and show that the “Australian problem” does not extend to all corners of the phonology.

Background 
Existing phonological surveys of Australian languages have focused on phoneme inventories, static phonotactics and stress patterns. However, to better understand the Australian problem we require more information, preferably both synchronic and diachronic, and thus a promising domain of investigation is morphophonemic alternations: synchronic phenomena which preserve a strong signal of prior changes.

Data 
The AusPhon-Alternations database is the first large scale survey of segmental morphophonemic alternations in Australian languages. Alternations are coded in a commensurate manner, irrespective of their description in source materials as ‘allomorphy’ or ‘(morpho)phonological rules’. In order to survey information from a wide band of time depths, we will not distinguish here between productive and nonproductive alternations, but focus instead on the alternations’ content. At time of writing, 80 linguistic varieties and ca. 1,500 alternations have been coded for.

Emerging findings

NO ‘AUSTRALIAN TYPE’  In Australia, segment inventories, phonotactic constraints and stress patterns show only minor variation across the vast majority of languages and language families. In contrast, there is no comparable, widespread sharing of segmental morphophonological alternations. The following patterns do recur across languages, but the rate of incidence is low.

1. STOP LENITION  A pattern of sonority-conditioned stop lenition, identified in earlier research, is not uncommon: stops alternate with glides or zero, with stops appearing after occlusives, and glides appearing after continuants.

2. CONSONANT ASSIMILATION  Assimilation in place and manner of articulation is rare, however this can be predicted given phonotactic factors. Namely, since phonotactic constraints typically permit only few sonority sequence types and place sequence types, and since geminates are generally not permitted, what would have been place assimilation typically results in complete deletion, as for example in /ɲn/ → /nn/ → /n/.
 
3. DELETION IN V+V CLUSTERS  Vowels + vowel clusters may simplify by deleting either vowel. This includes when the V+V cluster has been created by a foregoing consonant deletion, raising questions for the standard account in Optimality Theory.

Conclusions/perspective 
The typological homogeneity of Australian language phonologies does not extend to morphophonology. Nevertheless, our observations suggest new insights into those aspects of phonology which are highly uniform: the lenition of stops to glides is inventory-preserving; and assimilation is rare except when it feeds deletion, which preserves phonotactic patterns. Though these effects are small and infrequent, in the long run they may contribute to the temporal stability of the most widespread phonological patterns."
The indigenous languages of Australia are remarkable for their low level of phonological diversity. Phonemic inventories and phonotactic restrictions bear strong resemblances from coast to coast, ranging across more than two hundred... more
The indigenous languages of Australia are remarkable for their low level of phonological diversity. Phonemic inventories and phonotactic restrictions bear strong resemblances from coast to coast, ranging across more than two hundred languages in twenty-eight  families. In this talk I review the well-known points of commonality in Australian  consonant systems and introduce some new respects in which the languages are similar. I also raise the question of how the constrained phonological diversity of Australian  languages ought to figure in our conceptualisation of the relationship between possible  and probable languages – the ultimate object of linguistic theory, and attested  phonological diversity – our principal empirical source of premises for reasoning about that ultimate object.
Many Australian languages exhibit morphophonological alternations between a plosive alternant which appears after a preceding plosive or nasal, and a continuant or zero alternant which appears otherwise. This talk presents results of a... more
Many Australian languages exhibit morphophonological alternations between a plosive alternant which appears after a preceding plosive or nasal, and a continuant or zero alternant which appears otherwise. This talk presents results of a comparative study of such alternations among the Tangkic languages, providing an insight into how a set of morphophonological alternations which is common across Australia may evolve over time.
Phonology and fieldwork can most often be found interfacing within a methodological virtuous circle: the findings of fieldwork provide input to phonological analysis and theory, which in turn provide insightful questions to take back to... more
Phonology and fieldwork can most often be found interfacing within a methodological virtuous circle: the findings of fieldwork provide input to phonological analysis and theory, which in turn provide insightful questions to take back to the field.  In the case of endangered languages, however, this process can be cut off before even a single full cycle has been completed. In light of this, there is an important role to be played by the phonologist in designing fieldwork strategies which ensure (i) that the initial production of data is as rich as possible, even in the absence of input from advanced phonological analysis, and (ii) that such data is delivered in a ‘user friendly’ format for those who will provide the subsequent theoretical analysis ¬– thereby allowing the virtuous circle to be completed without unnecessary delay. This presentation reports on a recent attempt to implement such ideas within a documentation project carried out with the last speakers of the moribund Australian (non Pama-Nyungan) language, Kayardild.

Already in a highly precarious position, Kayardild is unlikely to survive much longer than five or ten more years.  When it ceases to be spoken, the entire Tangkic language family will have become extinct, and while this window of five or so years provides invaluable time for research, it is not long.  In response to this, features were built into the design of a documentation project run in 2005 with a view both to practical feasibility and to the production of data in a form as outlined above. Primary among these was the enrichment of interlinear text glosses through the addition of two tiers of  prosodic information; secondary was the adoption of a phonologically shrewd approach to vocabulary documentation.  Neither of these strategies required any particularly advanced phonological training on the part of the field researcher – that is, they should be relatively easy to incorporate into other projects – and despite their simplicity, they appear to have proven successful.
In the presentation then, I discuss the precise nature of the rhythmic and intonational transcriptions made for Kayardild, outline how they have already proven useful, and comment and how the methods could be extended to other field projects.  I also offer some observations on mundane but nevertheless important details which can impact on the effectiveness of phonological/phonetic data collection.

The talk should be of interest to any phonologist in a position to offer advice to fieldworkers on the collection of phonological data – that is, to most of us.
"From the perspective of languages on other continents, the phonological inventories of Australian languages are unusually similar. Given the large geographical distances and historical time depths involved, this fact suggests that the... more
"From the perspective of languages on other continents, the phonological inventories of Australian languages are unusually similar. Given the large geographical distances and historical time depths involved, this fact suggests that the languages’ phonological systems possess some particular kind of diachronic stability. This paper presents findings from the Australian Language Phonological Database pilot study, highlighting recurrent sound changes that have affected stop phonemes in languages throughout Australia (both Pama-Nyungan and non-Pama-Nyungan), and considers the question of how these might relate to a notion of diachronic stability in Australian language phonologies.

At least 10% of Australian languages exhibit some synchronic reflex of a set of changes in which stops have become continuants in the environment of a preceding and following liquid or (semi-)vowel. Several recurrent patterns are identifiable.

A greater preponderance of dorsals and labials undergo such changes, compared to coronals. Dorsal stops often undergo complete historical deletion, whereas labial stops tend to become labial-dorsal semivowels. Laminal palatal stops tend to become laminal palatal semivowels. Laminal dental stops appear to become approximants, but these are diachronically short-lived and tend to change further, into laminal palatal semivowels or laterals. Apical retroflex stops occasionally become retroflex approximants but also become laminal palatal semivowels. Apical alveolar stops occasionally become apical trills.

All of these changes are motivated relatively well in terms of our current understanding of the articulation of stops in Australian languages, and all of them lead to the creation of segments which are already found in almost every Australian language — they are thus ‘stable’ in terms of their systemic effects. Nevertheless, there are other patterns of change attested in Australia which notionally would also be motivated, yet which are rare or localised to particular regions. Implications of these observations are discussed.
"
In an analysis of the inflectional morphology of Kayardild (Tangkic), Round (2009) invokes a layer of representation intermediate between morphosyntax and underlying phonological form, identified as ‘morphomic’ following Aronoff (1994).... more
In an analysis of the inflectional morphology of Kayardild (Tangkic), Round (2009) invokes a layer of representation intermediate between morphosyntax and underlying phonological form, identified as ‘morphomic’ following Aronoff (1994). This paper clarifies the abstract properties of Round’s analysis and asks if and how it differs from some other architectures of morphological complexity.
Round’s analysis builds on Aronoff’s MORPHOME concept: a morphome is a category which figures systematically in the organization of a language’s morphology but is not isomorphic with any morphosyntactic, semantic or phonological category. Aronoff in fact discusses two kinds of morphome. The first kind classifies lexemes according to their patterns of inflection, e.g. classifying together lexemes of an inflectional class. This kind of category could be termed a RHIZOMORPHOME (ρM), literally ‘a morphome for roots’ following Stump’s (2002) argument that inflectional classes are actually properties of roots. The second kind of morphome classifies word forms according to their parts, e.g. classifying together the inflected and derived words of Latin which contain a ‘third root’ element. We may call this kind a MEROMORPHOME (µM), literally ‘a morphome for pieces’.
The morphomes of Round’s analysis are µM’s. Abstractly, a lexeme index L plus a partially ordered morphosytactic feature set σ map onto a stem S plus a partially ordered set of µM’s, which then map onto an underlying phonological form φ. The phonological form φ is composed of phono¬logical modifications P of a phonological stem π. This is shown in (1) where I use the operator ‘◦’ to generalise over various possible ways of applying P1...Pi to π. This enables (1) to pertain without loss of generality to both ordered-rule and constraint-based optimization models of morphophonology.
In Round’s notation, elements in morphomic representations such as (2a,b) map onto concatenative morphs as in (2c,d) and so it may appear that the model is inherently concat¬enative. However, once the architecture is re-expressed in the generalized manner of (1) it should be clear that the operations P could equally be non-concatenative. To generalize further, in (3) I make use of the operator ‘◦’ also in the morphomic representation. This underscores the fact that the morphomic representation is no more than a set of elements {µM1...µMk, S} related in a manner which is (potentially) transitive and asymmetrical. What then if anything is distinctive about Round’s (2009) architecture? Two central properties are the following.
The first is that the µM units in (3) are not atomic but are decomposed into matrices of features that capture further generalizations. In theoretic terms this elaborates Aronoff’s concept of a mero-morphome. Note it has already been proposed in network morphology that rhizomorphomic inflection classes are related via inheritance trees; feature matrices are somewhat more powerful.
The second is that individual units µM appear in various mappings in order to capture full or partial identities of form. By figuring in the morphomic representation of multiple cells of a paradigm (e.g., L,σ and L,τ where σ≠τ) they can capture syncretism. When appearing in cells of different lexemes’ paradigms (e.g., Li,σ and Lj,τ where i≠j) they can capture identities in the inflectional forms of e.g. nouns and verbs. When appearing in the expansion of stems as in (4) they can capture identities between inflectional and derivational morphology. They are thus more powerful than formalisms which are defined so as to express identities solely with one paradigm, or solely within the various paradigms of one lexeme.
A task for future research is to ascertain to what extent this additional architectural power is warranted, and in which other empirical or theoretical domains it can usefully be applied.
(1) L,σ → S, 〈µM1,µM2>µM3...>...µMk〉 → P1◦P2◦P3◦...◦Pi◦π = φ
(2) a. S-µPROP-µOBL  → c.  /π-kuɻu-in̪t̪a/
b. S-µPROP-µLOC  → d.  /π-kuɻu-ki/
(3) L,σ → µM1◦µM2◦µM3◦...◦µMk◦S → P1◦P2◦P3◦...◦Pi◦π = φ
(4) S → R-µMi-µMj, for root R.
Aronoff, M. 1994. Morphology by itself. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Round, E. 2009. Kayardild Morphology, Phonology and Morphosyntax. Yale PhD dissertation.
Stump, G. 2002. ‘Morphological and Syntactic Paradigms.’ Yearbook of Morphology 2001:147-180.
Gender systems are endlessly fascinating, from those where meaning determines gender (Bagvalal), those where it is dominant but leaves intriguing loopholes (Mian) to those where form has an important role (Russian). Now it is time to... more
Gender systems are endlessly fascinating, from those where meaning determines gender (Bagvalal), those where it is dominant but leaves intriguing loopholes (Mian) to those where form has an important role (Russian). Now it is time to integrate these systems into a fuller typology of nominal classification, taking in classifier systems as well as gender. Rethinking in this way leads us to take apart characteristics usually lumped together as defining gender, and those defining traditional classifiers. We then see that these characteristics combine in many ways. A canonical perspective proves helpful: we define the notion of canonical gender, and use this idealization as a baseline from which to calibrate the rich variety we find. It is then possible to approach the origin and nature of gender. Here Oceanic languages can provide a unique insight. Typically, a noun can occur with different possessive classifiers, depending on how the possessed item is used by the possessor: ‘my water (to drink)’ vs ‘my water (for something else)’. But in marked contrast, languages like North Ambrym (Vanuatu) typically have particular nouns occurring with one given classifier: water is just drinkable (Franjieh 2016). North Ambrym’s innovative system resembles a gender system: a noun takes a particular classifier regardless. We want to establish empirically whether gender systems can indeed emerge from possessive classifiers in this way. We must also uncover how and why languages would relinquish a useful, meaningful classificatory system, and move to a more rigid gender system. To this end we are running novel experiments to compare possessive classifier systems in six Oceanic languages of Vanuatu and New Caledonia, each with a different inventory size of classifiers — from two to twenty-three. Combining typology with psycholinguistics in this way promises to shed new light on how systems of nominal classification develop and function. The Oceanic data suggest that, in this instance, we find an interesting parallelism: diachronic change is running in the direction of canonicity.
Automated cognate alignment can help speed up the comparative method, however current implementations (List 2010, List et al 2018) are generic and phonetically course grained, so may miss detail and family-/area-specific information... more
Automated cognate alignment can help speed up the comparative method, however current implementations (List 2010, List et al 2018) are generic and phonetically course grained, so may miss detail and family-/area-specific information utilized in manual research. We demonstrate the advantages of adding family-specific historical phonological knowledge to automated processes. Using information on Australian sound changes (Dixon 2002) we assessed the alignments of 1,000 cognate sets across eight Gunwinyguan languages (Harvey 2003). Relative to a hand-curated gold standard, our aligner produced 25% fewer errors (36/1000) than the generic aligner (48/1000). Our method is straightforward and can be extended to other families.
For many revitalization projects, the only existing language data comes from word lists recorded by linguistically-naïve transcribers, yet there is a lack of standardized methodology to interpret these sources. We draw upon previous... more
For many revitalization projects, the only existing language data comes from word lists recorded by linguistically-naïve transcribers, yet there is a lack of standardized methodology to interpret these sources. We draw upon previous iterations of linguistic ‘reconstitution’ (Broadbent 1957; Dench 1999; Browne 2016) which themselves have drawn upon the historical-comparative method in order to transform the language data into a modern (i.e. phonemic) format, useable by linguists and language workers.
This paper aims to further refine the comparative reconsitution methodology by incorporating computer-assisted cognate alignment. This alignment takes identified orthographic ‘cognates’ as input and derives aligned correspondences. We tested our methodology on a collection of sources by linguistically-naïve English speakers recording Bunganditj (Pama-Nyungan, Australia: Blake 2003). Incorporating computational assistance dramatically reduces the duration of the reconstitution project, making it more suitable for revitalisation projects, while also increasing the accuracy of the results.
* Robots who read grammars * Jayden L. Macklin-Cordes, Nathaniel L. Blackbourne, Thomas J. Bott, Jacqueline Cook, T. Mark Ellison, Jordan Hollis, Edith E. Kirlew, Genevieve C. Richards, Sanle Zhao, Erich R. Round Poster presented at... more
* Robots who read grammars *
Jayden L. Macklin-Cordes, Nathaniel L. Blackbourne, Thomas J. Bott, Jacqueline Cook, T. Mark Ellison, Jordan Hollis, Edith E. Kirlew, Genevieve C. Richards, Sanle Zhao, Erich R. Round

Poster presented at CoEDL Fest 2017, Alexandra Park Conference Centre, Alexandra Headlands, QLD, Australia. Hosted by the University of Queensland. 6 February 2017.
https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4625248

* Abstract *
Linguistic typology has yet to undergo a computational revolution like that seen in other scientific endeavours. Nevertheless, we could soon be able to query the entire store of published knowledge on human languages when we do our research. To do so, knowledge must be represented in a machine-readable format. Here, we introduce a prototype ‘Grammar Harvester’, a set of processes for creating richly annotated, machine-readable versions of existing grammatical descriptions, starting from a scanned PDF. Further, we introduce ‘Finder’ and ‘Analyser’ Robots, scripts which automatically identify and compile information from harvested grammars using novel and existing ontologies of linguistic concepts.

* Author bio *
Jayden Macklin-Cordes is a PhD candidate at the Ancient Language Lab, University of Queensland. He is the lead investigator on CoEDL Transdisciplinary and Innovation Grant, 'A "data well" prototype for Sahul phonologies'. Erich Round (Ancient Language Lab director, UQ) and Mark Ellison (ANU) are collaborators on the same grant. Working hard on the project are Summer Research Scholars Sanle Zhao, Edith Kirlew, Thomas Bott and Nathaniel Blackbourne (all UQ), with further generous assistance from research assistants Genevieve Richards, Jordan Hollis, and Jacqueline Cook (all UQ).
Citation: Macklin-Cordes, J. L. & E. R. Round, 2016. Reflections of linguistic history in quantitative phonotactics. Paper presented at the Australian Linguistic Society Annual Conference, Monash University, Caulfield, Australia. 7... more
Citation:
Macklin-Cordes, J. L. & E. R. Round, 2016. Reflections of linguistic history in quantitative phonotactics. Paper presented at the Australian Linguistic Society Annual Conference, Monash University, Caulfield, Australia. 7 December 2016. Doi: https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.4299365

Abstract:
Advanced quantitative methods are at the cutting edge of historical linguistics, however these methods often ideally require many hundreds of data points per language. In order to generate reliable inferences at ever greater time depths, there is a need for typological datasets which are not only broader in coverage, but also contain a deeper store of information. We explore one avenue by extracting large numbers of high-definition phonotactic ‘traits’ per language. We show that these traits contain phylogenetic signal, thus demonstrating an important path towards high-powered methods of the near future.

Methodology: Languages may be compared in terms of which two-segment sequences they permit. Moreover, such biphones possess distinct lexical frequencies, which can also be compared. We examined whether such data contain information about family-tree structure, i.e., phylogenetic signal. Two standard statistics are used: D [1] tests coarse-grained biphone ‘permissibility’ data; and K [2] tests higher-definition transition probabilities.

We examined 2 subgroups of the Australian Pama-Nyungan family: 10 languages of Ngumpin-Yapa [3] and 7 of Yolngu [4], represented by phonemically-standardised lexicons from the CHIRILA database [5]. Phylogenetic signal is calculated with reference to phylogenies from C. Bowern (updated from [6]). Australian languages present a tough challenge, since phonotactically they are notoriously uniform [7­–9]. Moreover, Ngumpin-Yapa has some of the world’s highest borrowing rates [10–11]. Thus we hypothesized that the coarse-grained D test would fail. The key question is whether the high-definition K test succeeds.

Results: D attempts to reject two null hypotheses: that traits’ distributions are (A) too uniform to reveal structure present in the reference tree; and (B) random. We extracted 184 (Ngumpin-Yapa) and 164 (Yolngu) traits per language. We were surprised to reject both hypotheses for Yolngu (Stouffer’s Z>100, p=0.00): thus, even binary permissibility data revealed some phylogenetic signal. For N-Y only the second null hypothesis could be rejected (p=0.00), and further testing showed that when the subgroup’s outermost language was removed, even this failed. We conclude that binary phonotactic data contains weak phylogenetic signal at best; the Y result may represent statistical noise, and more subgroups should be tested.

K attempts to reject one null hypothesis: that no phylogenetic signal is present. A value K=0 represents random trait distribution relative to the reference tree; K=1 represents an exact match and K>1 indicates that outermost languages are even more distinct in the test data than in the reference tree. With 451 (Ngumpin-Yapa) and 541 (Yolngu) traits per language, we reject the null hypothesis in both subgroups (Stouffer’s Z=9.87; 17.6, p=0.00). In Ngumpin-Yapa, the confidence interval for K of [0.86, 0.92] indicates a very good match with the reference phylogeny, and in Yolngu, [1.15, 1.26] indicates an even stronger sorting of languages. Further testing, which removed the outermost language from both subgroups showed the result is stable: [0.81, 0.87] for Ngumpin-Yapa and [0.96, 1.00] for Yolngu.

Conclusion: As linguists attempt to up-scale efforts in quantitative historical linguistics, we demonstrate the significant potential of high-definition phonotactics, which permits the extraction of several hundred traits per language and has revealed phylogenetic signal in two Australian subgroups.


References:

[1]    S.A. Fritz and A. Purvis, “Selectivity in mammalian extinction risk and threat types: A new measure of phylogenetic signal strength in binary traits,” Conserv. Biol., vol. 24, no. 4., pp. 1042-1051, 2010.

[2]    S.P. Blomberg, T. Garland and A.R. Ives, “Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: Behavioural traits are more labile,” Evolution, vol. 57, no. 4, pp. 717-745, 2003.

[3]    P. McConvell and M. Laughren, “The Ngumpin-Yapa subgroup,” in Australian Languages: Classification and the comparative method, C. Bowern and H. Koch, Eds. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 2004, pp. 151-177.

[4]    Schebeck, Bernhard Dialect and Social Groupings in North East Arnhem Land, typescript, Australian Institute of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Studies Library, Canberra, 1968.

[5]    C. Bowern, “Chrila: Contemporary and Historical Resources for Indigenous Languages of Australia,” Language Documentation and Conservation, vol. 10 http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/ldc/

[6]    C. Bowern and Q.D. Atkinson, “Computational phylogenetics and the internal structure of Pama-Nyungan,” Language, vol. 88, no. 4, pp. 817-845, 2012.

[7]    R.M.W. Dixon, The Languages of Australia, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1980.

[8]    P.J. Hamilton, “Phonetic constraints and markedness in the phonotactics of Australian languages,” Ph.D. dissertation, University of Toronto, 1996.

[9]    B. Baker, “Word structure in Australian languages,” in The Languages and Linguistics of Australia: A comprehensive guide, H. Koch and R. Nordlinger, Eds. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, 2014, pp. 139-214.

[10] C. Bowern, et al., “Does lateral transmission obscure inheritance in hunter-gatherer languages?” PLoS One, 2011: e25195.

[11] P. McConvell, “Loanwords in Gurindji, a Pama-Nyungan language of Australia,” in Loanwords in the world's languages: A comparative handbook, M. Haspelmath and U. Tadmore, Eds. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 2009, pp. 790–822.

[12] Y. Benjamini and Y. Hochberg, “Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing,” J. R. Stat. Soc. Series B (Stat. Methodol.), vol. 57, no. 1, pp. 289-300, 1995.
Research Interests:
We report on a project which has created a digital version of lexical material on approx. 70 language varieties of Cape York, from the archival records of Bruce Sommer [1]. Our focus here is on methodology. Background & aims: Great... more
We report on a project which has created a digital version of lexical material on approx. 70 language varieties of Cape York, from the archival records of Bruce Sommer [1]. Our focus here is on methodology.

Background & aims:
Great strides have been made in preparing the lexicons of Australian languages in digitally readable and accessible form, however a notable gap so far is Cape York [2]. Bruce Sommer deposited lexical, grammatical and textual materials on some 70 language varieties of central and southern Cape York, comprising 4,950 pages of fieldnotes and summaries, and 203 audio tapes. Our aim was to key in Sommer’s handwritten and printed lexical materials, as a first step in the digital representation and eventual audio time-alignment of his invaluable archive.

Materials:
Fryer Library digitised Sommer’s print materials in 2014 and tapes in 2015. We identified 1,520 pages of lexical material. These wordlists range in length from 2 entries to 2635 (mean 485, median 255). Many are numbered, following the Hale–O’Grady 100-item list.

Methods:
Our work plan centred on simultaneous and collaborative data entry. Two researchers entered the same wordlist simultaneously into a Google spreadsheet, where the other’s activity is also visible. Each worker focussed on either the vernacular or English, but also provided constant checking of the other’s work, and assistance when necessary. The spreadsheet contained columns for: speaker, language, tape number, subheadings, page number, language form, notes on language form, English gloss, notes on English gloss, other text and notes on other text. Additional columns were added if wordlists become more complex: language form corrections, number, addi- tional language form columns for lists with two vernacular languages.

Challenges:
1. Legibility of handwriting was a challenge. To improve accuracy, researchers examined illegible entries together to reach agreement; if needed, other wordlists were consulted, to see if a word appeared elsewhere with a similar form. In rare cases where neither of these solutions worked, a note was entered.
2. Sommer used many abbreviations. These were gradually deciphered as our familiarity increased.
3. Some pages contained extensive corrections, annotations and/or margin notes; some had multiple languages or speakers. Extra columns were added for those documents.
4. Most of the materials were in IPA. This was entered using a convenient set of as hoc conventions to enable fast data entry, and then transposed into IPA afterwards. Having two researchers dealing collaboratively with challenges led to rapid and effective problem solving.
Analysis Cape York is a notoriously complex region [3]. Cross-linguistic datasets such as Sommer’s lexicons will make possible automated analyses which can detect diffuse patterns which challenge the observational and memory limitations of human linguists. We present some initial examples, including automated phylogenetic analysis [4]; network analysis [5]; and admixture analysis [6]. These do not replace expert manual analysis, but can increase productivity by rapidly highlighting areas deserving particular attention.

Methodological recommendations:
We cannot recommend strongly enough the method of collaborative data entry for this kind of data, which enables quick and effective detection and correction of data entry errors. It makes the task more collaborative, and hence enjoyable.


References:
[1] Sommer, B. 2003. Papers, 1964–2003 (item number UQFL476), Fryer Library, St Lucia.
[2] Bowern, C. 2016. Chirila: Contemporary and Historical Resources for the Indigenous Languages of Australia. Language Documentation and Conservation. Vol 10.
[3] P.Sutton(ed.) 1975. Languages of Cape York, Canberra: AIAS.
[4] Blomberg, S.P., T. Garland & A.R. Ives. 2003. Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: Behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution 57:717-45.
[5] Bryant, D., & Moulton, V. 2004. Neighbor-net: an agglomerative method for the construction of phylogenetic networks. Molecular biology and evolution, 21(2), 255-265.
[6] Pritchard, J. K., Stephens, M., & Donnelly, P. (2000). Inference of population structure using multilocus genotype data. Genetics, 155(2), 945-959.
Research Interests:
Database and dataset descriptions for Diachronic Atlas of Comparative Linguistics Online. Data is available on https://diacl.ht.lu.se/
Research Interests:
In “Where’s phonology in typology?” Larry Hyman (2007) observed that phonology, a discipline with an abiding interest in typology and much to offer it, had only a slender profile in publications representing the discipline of typology... more
In “Where’s phonology in typology?” Larry Hyman (2007) observed that phonology, a discipline with an abiding interest in typology and much to offer it, had only a slender profile in publications representing the discipline of typology itself. In the ensuing years, not much changed (Hyman 2017). The Oxford handbook of linguistic typology (Song 2010), for example, grants phonology a single chapter out of thirty, and likewise the The Cambridge handbook of linguistic typology (Aikhenvald and Dixon 2017). The result has been a deficit of extended, synthesizing work. However, with Matthew K. Gordon’s Phonological typology, published in the new series Oxford Surveys in Phonology and Phonetics, the landscape changes. Here we have a serious overview of the field with enough room for richly informative data to join critically with multiple theoretical viewpoints, supplemented with key references and tidy summaries. Moreover, Gordon delivers on Hyman’s (2007) promise that phonology can offer particular insights for the theory of typology in general, especially around the role of levels of analysis, a topic which I take up in the discussion below. Consequently, this is a book which should be read not only by all phonologists, but by theoretically minded typologists of all subfields.
Research Interests:
This article investigates the evolutionary and spatial dynamics of typological characters in 117 Indo-European languages. We partition types of change (i.e., gain or loss) for each variant according to whether they bring about a... more
This article investigates the evolutionary and spatial dynamics of typological characters in 117 Indo-European languages. We partition types of change (i.e., gain or loss) for each variant according to whether they bring about a simplification in morphosyntactic patterns that must be learned, whether they are neutral (i.e., neither simplifying nor introducing complexity) or whether they introduce a more complex pattern. We find that changes which introduce complexity show significantly less areal signal (according to a metric we devise) than changes which simplify and neutral changes, but we find no significant differences between the latter two groups. This result is compatible with a scenario where certain types of parallel change are more likely to be mediated by advergence and contact between proximate speech communities, while other developments are due purely to drift and are largely independent of intercultural contact.