Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Skip to main content
This Element explores multifaceted linkages between feeding and relationship formation based on ethnographic case studies in Morocco, Madagascar, Sri Lanka, Taiwan, and Costa Rica. Research demonstrates that there are many culturally... more
This Element explores multifaceted linkages between feeding
and relationship formation based on ethnographic case studies in
Morocco, Madagascar, Sri Lanka, Taiwan, and Costa Rica. Research
demonstrates that there are many culturally valued ways of feeding
children, contradicting the idea of a single universally optimal feeding
standard. It demonstrates further that, in many parts of the world,
feeding plays a central role in bonding and relationship formation,
something largely overlooked in current developmental theories.
Analysis shows that feeding contributes to relationship formation
through what we call proximal, transactional, and distal dimensions.
This Element argues that feeding practices can lead to qualitatively
distinct forms of relationships. It has important theoretical and practical
implications, calling for the expansion of attachment theory to include
feeding and body-centered caregiving and significant changes to
global interventions currently based on “responsive feeding.” This title
is also available as Open Access on Cambridge Core.
Der Bevölkerungsanteil mit vietnamesischer Migrationsgeschichte in Deutschland wird häufig auf zwei Einwanderungsgruppen zurückgeführt: sogenannte Boat People, die Ende der 1970er und Anfang 1980er Jahre von der BRD als... more
Der Bevölkerungsanteil mit vietnamesischer Migrationsgeschichte in Deutschland wird häufig auf zwei Einwanderungsgruppen zurückgeführt: sogenannte Boat People, die Ende der 1970er und Anfang 1980er Jahre von der BRD als Kontingentflüchtlinge aufgenommen wurden (Su & Sanko 2017), und Vertragsarbeiter, die in den 1980er Jahren in die DDR kamen (Dennis 2005). Diese beiden Gruppen haben insbesondere aufgrund der gegensätzlichen Migrationsregime und vielschichtigen Verflechtungen mit der Geschichte von Teilung und Wiedervereinigung beider Länder einige Aufmerksamkeit in den Medien, in Kunst und Kultur sowie der Forschung erhalten. Migration aus Vietnam nach Deutschland ist allerdings kein abgeschlossener Prozess. Wie wir in diesem Beitrag zeigen, ist spätestens seit der Jahrtausendwende eine stetig anwachsende Zuwanderung zu verzeichnen. Neue vietnamesi-sche Migrant*innen in diesem Sinne dürften mittlerweile gar die Mehr-heit innerhalb der deutsch-vietnamesischen Bevölkerung ausmachen.
The chapter presents an ethnographic case study about the anger repertoire in a rural community of Madagascar. Based on emotion narratives, it first depicts the meaning and internal differentiation of the anger vocabulary in use among... more
The chapter presents an ethnographic case study about the anger repertoire in a rural community of Madagascar. Based on emotion narratives, it first depicts the meaning and internal differentiation of the anger vocabulary in use among this particular population. Then, it reconstructs how children acquire and learn to differentiate the anger emotions of their community. According to the analysis presented here, the conceptual differentiation of anger is clearly based on particular, shared patterns of social relationships and interactions as well as on the affective dynamics they engender. Key Words: emotion, affect, anger, childhood, socialization, child development, Madagascar, cross-cultural, anthropology, cultural psychology
Many claim that political deliberation has become exceedingly affective, and hence, destabilizing. The authors of this book revisit that assumption. While recognizing that significant changes are occurring, these authors also point out... more
Many claim that political deliberation has become exceedingly affective, and hence, destabilizing. The authors of this book revisit that assumption. While recognizing that significant changes are occurring, these authors also point out the limitations of turning to contemporary democratic theory to understand and unpack these shifts. They propose, instead, to reframe this debate by deploying the analytic framework of affective societies, which highlights how affect and emotion are present in all aspects of the social. What changes over time and place are the modes and calibrations of affective and emotional registers. With this line of thinking, the authors are able to gesture towards a new outline of the political.
Research Interests:
Dieser Beitrag entwirft eine Typologie der Wut aus kulturvergleichender Perspektive. Auf der Basis von emotionsanthropologischer Forschung werden drei Formen von Wut beschrieben: Sanktionswut, Vergeltungswut und appellierende Wut. Diese... more
Dieser Beitrag entwirft eine Typologie der Wut aus kulturvergleichender Perspektive. Auf der Basis von emotionsanthropologischer Forschung werden drei Formen von Wut beschrieben: Sanktionswut, Vergeltungswut und appellierende Wut. Diese Wuttypen sind in verschiedenen kulturellen Kontexten unterschiedlich ausdifferenziert.
Wie erlernen Kinder Emotionen und welche Rolle spielt dabei ihre soziale und kulturelle Umwelt? Gabriel Scheidecker untersucht die – bisher primär in westlichen Kontexten erforschte – Sozialisation von Emotionen erstmals in Madagaskar.... more
Wie erlernen Kinder Emotionen und welche Rolle spielt dabei ihre soziale und kulturelle Umwelt? Gabriel Scheidecker untersucht die – bisher primär in westlichen Kontexten erforschte – Sozialisation von Emotionen erstmals in Madagaskar. Auf der Basis einer 15-monatigen Feldforschung in einer ländlichen Region der Insel beschreibt er detailliert die emotionalen Erfahrungen von Kindern in Verbindung mit den Erziehungsidealen und -praktiken ihrer Bezugspersonen. Im Fokus steht die Ausbildung einer kulturspezifischen moralischen Furcht gegenüber den Eltern sowie die feine Ausdifferenzierung von Wut. Damit erweitert der Band die Forschung zur Emotionssozialisation um eine kulturanthropologische Perspektive.
The science and practice of Early Childhood Development (ECD) rely heavily on research from the Euro-American middle class—a minority of the world’s population—and research in or from the majority world is severely under-represented. This... more
The science and practice of Early Childhood Development (ECD) rely heavily on research from the Euro-American middle class—a minority of the world’s population—and research in or from the majority world is severely under-represented. This problem has been acknowledged in ECD, an applied field aiming to assess and improve child development globally, and in the related fields of global health and developmental sciences. Thus, now is the time to search for effective pathways towards global representation. To date, most calls for change within ECD and related fields have focused on various aspects of knowledge production and publication. Although more majority world research is certainly needed, we should work equally on the reception of existing research. A large body of research on childhood in the majority world already exists (eg, in anthropology, cultural psychology, and indigenous psychology), but majority world research is almost entirely absent in ECD. The under-representation of majority world research in ECD cannot simply be blamed on the scarcity of research, however. This under-representation is also perpetuated by the exclusion of existing, accessible, and highly relevant majority world research from the dominant academic discourses—in other words, by epistemic exclusion. If epistemic exclusion in the field of ECD continues, the existence of more majority world research will not reduce the current minority world biases in ECD research. Ending epistemic exclusion is therefore an essential step towards a truly global ECD practice.
This Element explores multi-faceted linkages between feeding and relationship formation based on ethnographic case studies in Morocco, Madagascar, Sri Lanka, Taiwan, and Costa Rica. Research demonstrates that there are many culturally... more
This Element explores multi-faceted linkages between feeding and relationship formation based on ethnographic case studies in Morocco, Madagascar, Sri Lanka, Taiwan, and Costa Rica. Research demonstrates that there are many culturally valued ways of feeding children, contradicting the idea of a single universally optimal feeding standard. It demonstrates further that in many parts of the world, feeding plays a central role in bonding and relationship formation, something largely overlooked in current developmental theories. Analysis shows that feeding contributes to relationship formation through what we call proximal, transactional, and distal dimensions. This Element argues that feeding practices can lead to qualitatively distinct forms of relationships. It has important theoretical and practical implications, calling for the expansion of attachment theory to include feeding and body-centered caregiving and significant changes to global interventions currently based on 'respon...
Research on childhood in anthropology and neighboring disciplines has continuously broadened the range of the social partners that are considered relevant for young children's development-from parents to other caregivers, siblings, and... more
Research on childhood in anthropology and neighboring disciplines has continuously broadened the range of the social partners that are considered relevant for young children's development-from parents to other caregivers, siblings, and peers. Yet most studies as well as interventions in early childhood still focus exclusively on parents, who are presumed to be the most significant socializing agents. Objecting to such a hierarchical understanding of the social world of children, I propose a complementarity view. Rather than being linearly ranked in a hierarchy of significance, children's social partners may complement each other by providing different but equally significant experiences. My suggestions are based on an ethnographic study in a rural community in Madagascar. Focusing on children in the first 3 years of life, I explore the full range of their social partners and the respective experiences they provide. Caregivers focus on children's physical needs and aim to keep them in a calm emotional state, while other young related children are the most crucial partners when it comes to play, face-to-face interaction, and the exchange of intense emotions. These complementary roles, I argue, lead to the parallel formation of two distinct socioemotional modes: a hierarchical one and an egalitarian one.
Aus emotionsanthropologischer Perspektive werden diskrete Emotionen wie Arger, Zorn oder Emporung in der Regel als kultur- und sprachspezifische Auspragungen von Emotionalitat betrachtet. Entsprechend setzen die meisten... more
Aus emotionsanthropologischer Perspektive werden diskrete Emotionen wie Arger, Zorn oder Emporung in der Regel als kultur- und sprachspezifische Auspragungen von Emotionalitat betrachtet. Entsprechend setzen die meisten Emotionsethnografien an dem im jeweiligen Forschungskontext gebrauchlichen Emotionsvokabular an, dessen Bedeutung analysiert und in die Publikationssprache ubersetzt wird. So umschreibt etwa Lutz das auf der mikronesischen Insel Ifaluk gelaufige Emotionskonzept song als eine Form von righteous anger und weist zugleich darauf hin, dass im Englischen kein exaktes Pendant existiere (vgl. 1988).
Global Early Childhood Development (ECD)-an applied field with the aim to improve the "brain structure and function" of future generations in the global South-has moved to the center of international development. Global ECD rests heavily... more
Global Early Childhood Development (ECD)-an applied field with the aim to improve the "brain structure and function" of future generations in the global South-has moved to the center of international development. Global ECD rests heavily on evidence claims about widespread cognitive, social, and emotional deficits in the global South and the benefits of changing parenting practices in order to optimize early childhood development. We challenge these claims on the grounds that the leading ECD literature excludes research from anthropology, cultural psychology, and related fields that could provide crucial insights about childrearing and children's development in the targeted communities. We encourage anthropologists and other scholars with ethnographic expertise on childhood to critically engage with global ECD. To facilitate such an endeavor, this article sketches the history, scientific claims, and interventions of global ECD, points out the critical potential of ethnographic research, and suggests strategies to make ethnography more relevant.

KEYWORDS early childhood development, parenting interventions, international development, applied research
An estimated 250 million children under 5 years in low-income and middle-income countries (LMICs) are considered to be at risk of not achieving their full developmental potential because of inadequate care. This assessment was crucial for... more
An estimated 250 million children under 5 years in low-income and middle-income countries (LMICs) are considered to be at risk of not achieving their full developmental potential because of inadequate care. This assessment was crucial for establishing the Nurturing Care Framework, a roadmap for improving early childhood development globally. Although the number is still based on proxy measures (stunting and poverty rates), newer research draws directly on indicators of nurturing care, provided by UNICEF's Multiple Indicator Cluster Surveys. On the basis of these data, in The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health Dana McCoy and colleagues claimed that the problem is even bigger: they calculated that 74·6% of children in LMICs aged 3–4 years do not even receive minimally adequate nurturing care. This figure increases to 92·1% for sub-Saharan Africa and to 99·5% in Chad.
This chapter presents an alternative view to classic attachment theory and research, arguing for systematic, ethnographically informed, approaches to the study of child development. It begins with the observation that the attachment... more
This chapter presents an alternative view to classic attachment theory and research, arguing for systematic, ethnographically informed, approaches to the study of child development. It begins with the observation that the attachment relationships children develop are locally determined and insists that these features of attachment can only be captured through observing, talking with, and listening to local people as they go about living their lives, including caring for children. It reviews the profound ways in which child care around the world differs from the Western model, upon which attachment theory was founded and myriad recommendations have been derived. This worldwide account perspective of child care is profusely illustrated with ethnographic examples. Network theory is then discussed: from the full range of social networks to relational ones (i.e., smaller sets of individuals to whom children may become attached). The chapter considers attachment theorists’ resistance to t...
Attachment theory has its roots in an ethnocentric complex of ideas, longstanding in the United States, under the rubric of “intensive mothering.” Among these various approaches and programs, attachment theory has had an inordinate and... more
Attachment theory has its roots in an ethnocentric complex of ideas, longstanding in the United States, under the rubric of “intensive mothering.” Among these various approaches and programs, attachment theory has had an inordinate and wide-ranging influence on a wide range of professions concerned with children (family therapy, education, the legal system, and public policy, the medical profession, etc.) inside and outside the United States. This chapter looks critically at how attachment theory has been applied in a variety of contexts and discusses its influence on parenting. It examines the tension distortion that often results when research findings are translated into actual applications or programs, ignoring any particularities of cultural context. It describes how attachment theory has been used as the basis for child-rearing manuals and has influenced programs and policies more directly, to form legal decisions that affect families, as well as to develop public policy and p...
This article explores ethical issues raised by parenting interventions implemented in communities in low- to middle-income countries (LMICs) with rural, subsistence lifestyles. Many of these interventions foster “positive parenting... more
This article explores ethical issues raised by parenting interventions implemented in communities in low- to middle-income countries (LMICs) with rural, subsistence lifestyles. Many of these interventions foster “positive parenting practices” to improve children’s chances of fulfilling their developmental potential. The practices are derived from attachment theory and presented as the universal standard of good care. But attachment-based parenting is typical primarily of people living Western lifestyles and runs counter to the different ways many people with other lifestyles care for their children given what they want for them. Thus, such parenting interventions involve encouraging caregivers to change their practices and views, usually with little understanding of how such changes affect child, family, and community. This undermines researchers’ and practitioners’ ability to honor promises to uphold ethic codes of respect and beneficence. Support for this claim is provided by comp...
This article examines the parent intervention program evaluated by Weber et al. (2017) and argues that there are scientific and ethical problems with such intervention efforts in applied developmental science. Scientifically, these... more
This article examines the parent intervention program evaluated by Weber et al. (2017) and argues that there are scientific and ethical problems with such intervention efforts in applied developmental science. Scientifically, these programs rely on data from a small and narrow sample of the world's population; assume the existence of fixed developmental pathways; and pit scientific knowledge against indigenous knowledge. The authors question the critical role of talk as solely providing the rich cognitive stimulation important to school success, and the critical role of primary caregivers as teachers of children's verbal competency. Ethically, these programs do not sufficiently explore how an intervention in one aspect of child care will affect the community's culturally organized patterns of child care.
ABSTRACT Although much has been written about “socialization” and “emotion” in different cultures, these two topics have rarely been systematically combined and analyzed from the perspective of ontogenetic development. Drawing on... more
ABSTRACT Although much has been written about “socialization” and “emotion” in different cultures, these two topics have rarely been systematically combined and analyzed from the perspective of ontogenetic development. Drawing on approaches from developmental psychology and cultural anthropology, we examine how cultural models of emotion and their corresponding emotionally arousing child-rearing practices lead to culture-specific pathways of emotion development among the Bara (Madagascar) and the Tao (Taiwan). We focus especially on socializing emotions, which we define in terms of their function to orient individuals toward cultural norms and values and to inculcate social conformity. Based on empirical data gathered in long-term ethnographic field studies, we provide evidence that the Bara emphasize “fear” and the Tao “anxiety” as focal socializing emotions.
ABSTRACT This article addresses the interdependency between child-rearing goals and values, emotionally arousing child-rearing practices, and the socialization and development of so-called socializing emotions. The latter are assigned a... more
ABSTRACT This article addresses the interdependency between child-rearing goals and values, emotionally arousing child-rearing practices, and the socialization and development of so-called socializing emotions. The latter are assigned a general psychological control function that enables children to adjust their behavior and emotions to the normative prescriptions of their culture. It is assumed that they are inculcated by means of emotionally arousing strategies such as frightening, corporal punishment, mocking, shaming but also praising, encouraging, or cherishing (Quinn, 200539. Quinn , N. 2005. Universals of child rearing. Anthropological Theory, 5: 477–516. [CrossRef]View all references) and that—in line with Vygotsky's genetic law of development—they become internalized so effectively that they can exert their control function already without prior disciplining.
This article examines the parent intervention program evaluated by Weber et al. (2017) and argues that there are scientific and ethical problems with such intervention efforts in applied developmental science. Scientifically , these... more
This article examines the parent intervention program evaluated by Weber et al. (2017) and argues that there are scientific and ethical problems with such intervention efforts in applied developmental science. Scientifically , these programs rely on data from a small and narrow sample of the world's population; assume the existence of fixed developmental pathways; and pit scientific knowledge against indigenous knowledge. The authors question the critical role of talk as solely providing the rich cognitive stimulation important to school success, and the critical role of primary caregivers as teachers of children's verbal competency. Ethically, these programs do not sufficiently explore how an intervention in one aspect of child care will affect the commu-nity's culturally organized patterns of child care.
Many claim that political deliberation has become exceedingly affective, and hence, destabilizing. The authors of this book revisit that assumption. While recognizing that significant changes are occurring, these authors also point out... more
Many claim that political deliberation has become exceedingly affective, and hence, destabilizing. The authors of this book revisit that assumption. While recognizing that significant changes are occurring, these authors also point out the limitations of turning to contemporary democratic theory to understand and unpack these shifts. They propose, instead, to reframe this debate by deploying the analytic framework of affective societies, which highlights how affect and emotion are present in all aspects of the social. What changes over time and place are the modes and calibrations of affective and emotional registers. With this line of thinking, the authors are able to gesture towards a new outline of the political.
This article considers claims of Mesman et al. (2017) that sensitive responsiveness as defined by Ainsworth, while not uniformly expressed across cultural contexts, is universal. Evidence presented demonstrates that none of the components... more
This article considers claims of Mesman et al. (2017) that sensitive responsiveness as defined by Ainsworth, while not uniformly expressed across cultural contexts, is universal. Evidence presented demonstrates that none of the components of sensitive responsiveness (i.e., which partner takes the lead, whose point of view is primary , and the turn-taking structure of interactions) or warmth are universal. Mesman and colleagues' proposal that sensitive responsiveness is " providing for infant needs " is critiqued. Constructs concerning caregiver quality must be embedded within a nexus of cultural logic, including caregiving practices, based on ecologically valid childrearing values and beliefs. Sensitive responsiveness, as defined by Mesman and attachment theorists, is not universal. Attachment theory and cultural or cross-cultural psychology are not built on common ground.
This article considers claims of Mesman et al. (2017) that sensitive responsiveness as defined by Ainsworth, while not uniformly expressed across cultural contexts, is universal. Evidence presented demonstrates that none of the components... more
This article considers claims of Mesman et al. (2017) that sensitive responsiveness as defined by Ainsworth, while not uniformly expressed across cultural contexts, is universal. Evidence presented demonstrates that none of the components of sensitive responsiveness (i.e., which partner takes the lead, whose point of view is primary, and the turn-taking structure of interactions) or warmth are universal. Mesman and colleagues' proposal that sensitive responsiveness is "providing for infant needs" is critiqued. Constructs concerning caregiver quality must be embedded within a nexus of cultural logic, including caregiving practices, based on ecologically valid childrearing values and beliefs. Sensitive responsiveness, as defined by Mesman and attachment theorists, is not universal. Attachment theory and cultural or cross-cultural psychology are not built on common ground.
Für vietnamesische Migrant*innen in Deutschland stellt die Erziehung ihrer Kinder eine besondere Herausforderung dar. Neben der allgemeinen Elternaufgabe, ihre Kinder nach bestem Wissen und Können großzuziehen, stehen sie in besonderem... more
Für vietnamesische Migrant*innen in Deutschland stellt die Erziehung ihrer Kinder eine besondere Herausforderung dar. Neben der allgemeinen Elternaufgabe, ihre Kinder nach bestem Wissen und Können großzuziehen, stehen sie in besonderem Maße vor der Herausforderung, ihre Erziehung mit den für sie neuen Bedingungen, Normen und Erwartungen im Ankunftsland abzustimmen. Eine zentrale Rolle hierbei spielen die Hilfen zur Erziehung, ein staatlich organisiertes System mit der Aufgabe, Eltern durch Erziehungsberatung, sozialpädagogische Familienhilfe und andere Hilfeformen zu unterstützen und gleichzeitig über das Kindeswohl zu wachen. In diesem Beitrag setzen wir uns mit der Frage auseinander, wie vietnamesische Eltern in Berlin die Erziehungshilfe und ihre einzelnen Angebote erleben und welche Einstellungen und Emotionen sie diesen entgegenbringen. Ausgangspunkt ist hierbei unsere Beobachtung, dass vietnamesische Eltern der Erziehungshilfe häufig misstrauisch gegenüberstehen. Dies ist insbesondere deshalb bemerkenswert, als viele Eltern mit dem primären Ziel aus Vietnam nach Deutschland gekommen sind, ihren Kindern hier eine bessere Bildung zu ermöglichen. Warum sollten Eltern also Unterstützung bei einer Aufgabe ablehnen, der sie selbst äußerste Priorität einräumen? Wie lässt sich ihre Skepsis diesem institutionellen Feld gegenüber verstehen?
The present study investigated the impact of young adolescents’ learning environment on their culturally mediated view of themselves as expressed in their self-drawings. The sample consisted of 83 young adolescents, 35 male and 48 female,... more
The present study investigated the impact of young adolescents’ learning environment on their culturally mediated view of themselves as expressed in their self-drawings. The sample consisted of 83 young adolescents, 35 male and 48 female, aged 10 to 14 years with an overall average of 12 years living in three diverse ecosocial contexts within the Southern region of Madagascar: 28 participants were recruited in rural villages, 14 adolescents lived in a small town, and 41 adolescents were raised in a large city. The participants did not differ in age or gender distribution. The analyses revealed significant differences in the adolescents’ emotional expressions, drawing styles, visual appearances, and figure sizes in their self-depictions, which were in line with ecosocial variations in their learning environments. The findings are interpreted in light of the participants’ varying socialization experiences, access to formal education, and exposure to modern media and a Western lifestyle.
Research on socialization and ontogeny of emotions has been widely neglected in social and cultural anthropology. In contrast, developmental psychologists are occupied intensively with this subject but the majority of their studies focus... more
Research on socialization and ontogeny of emotions has been widely neglected in social and cultural anthropology. In contrast, developmental psychologists are occupied intensively with this subject but the majority of their studies focus on Euro-American societies and thus do not explain intercultural differences. In this article we compare the socialization of emotions in two non-western societies: the Bara in Madagascar and the Tao on the Taiwanese Island of Lanyu. It will be illustrated how folk models of person, emotion, and development interrelate with local child rearing practices. In both societies sanctioning strategies are used by care-givers who operate with high levels of emotional arousal. While “fear” is induced among the Bara, in the Tao’s case the evoked emotions are “anxiety” and “shame”. An important question, among others, is to what extent these “socializing emotions” play a role in the development of a culture-specific emotional repertoire.
In this contribution we 1. describe the photo-voice technique and how we adapted it for the study of feelings, emotion, and affect, 2. outline some of the most striking results of our study and 3. reflect on the intersection of narration... more
In this contribution we 1. describe the photo-voice technique and how we adapted it for the study of feelings, emotion, and affect, 2. outline some of the most striking results of our study and 3. reflect on the intersection of narration and visualization. Within the context of a school study we provided more than 100 students of different age grades (9-16 years) with small digital cameras and asked them to “picture their feelings”, that is to take some photos which show or symbolize what and who triggers emotions in them. They got two weeks to perform this task, so that they had enough time to reflect on it. Afterwards the children and adolescents were requested to select the 15 most important photos out of their sample and were interviewed about these pictures. The method turned out to be extremely efficient. In relation to the photos symbolizing the situations, objects and persons which are key to their daily affective experiences, the students produced dense narrations about their feelings.
This article examines the parent intervention program evaluated by Weber et al. (2017) and argues that there are scientific and ethical problems with such intervention efforts in applied developmental science. Scientifically , these... more
This article examines the parent intervention program evaluated by Weber et al. (2017) and argues that there are scientific and ethical problems with such intervention efforts in applied developmental science. Scientifically , these programs rely on data from a small and narrow sample of the world's population; assume the existence of fixed developmental pathways; and pit scientific knowledge against indigenous knowledge. The authors question the critical role of talk as solely providing the rich cognitive stimulation important to school success, and the critical role of primary caregivers as teachers of children's verbal competency. Ethically, these programs do not sufficiently explore how an intervention in one aspect of child care will affect the commu-nity's culturally organized patterns of child care. The reliance on parent intervention programs-such as the one described by Weber, Fernald, and Diop (2017)-to improve parenting practices and thus children's developmental achievements is a growing trend in applied developmental science. We find this trend alarming for both scientific and ethical reasons. These parenting programs rarely pay attention to the conceptual and methodological assumptions underlying the research and the translation of research findings on which they are based. The same can be said of the way these programs are evaluated. The assumptions that guide these endeavors reflect values and practices of a small percentage of the world's people: those who live Following Morelli, author names are listed in alphabetical order.
This article considers claims of Mesman et al. (2017) that sensitive responsiveness as defined by Ainsworth, while not uniformly expressed across cultural contexts, is universal. Evidence presented demonstrates that none of the components... more
This article considers claims of Mesman et al. (2017) that sensitive responsiveness as defined by Ainsworth, while not uniformly expressed across cultural contexts, is universal. Evidence presented demonstrates that none of the components of sensitive responsiveness (i.e., which partner takes the lead, whose point of view is primary , and the turn-taking structure of interactions) or warmth are universal. Mesman and colleagues' proposal that sensitive responsiveness is "providing for infant needs" is critiqued. Constructs concerning caregiver quality must be embedded within a nexus of cultural logic, including caregiving practices, based on ecologically valid childrearing values and beliefs. Sensitive responsiveness, as defined by Mesman and attachment theorists, is not universal. Attachment theory and cultural or cross-cultural psychology are not built on common ground.
Attachment refers to an enduring affectional bond of humans to particular others, whether individual or collective, as well as to non-human actors such as animals, material possessions, places, or spiritual beings. Attachments are... more
Attachment refers to an enduring affectional bond of humans to particular others, whether individual or collective, as well as to non-human actors such as animals, material possessions, places, or spiritual beings. Attachments are distinguished by their tendency to persist over time and across contexts and their profound emotional and affective significance. The concept of attachment allows attending to the diachronic dimension of emotional and affective relationships from the perspective of individual actors. As a fundamental precondition for human existence and the development of all major capacities, attachments are formed from birth onwards. Although early attachments may persist for decades or even a lifetime, individuals usually also lose attachment figures and turn toward new ones over the course of their life. The temporal dimension of attachment transcends particular bonds, since experiences in preceding attachment relationships function as dispositions for future attachments (affective disposition). Such dispositions for attachment are theorized as internal, affective-cognitive working models of the self in relation to particular others. They evolve through past relational experiences and guide ongoing interactions in existing attachment relationships as well as engagement in future relationships, which in turn provide new experiences. Thus, the concept of attachment accounts for both continuity and transformation across the course of life. Attachments are central for affective and emotional processes in several ways. To begin with, attachments themselves can be considered affectional bonds. This becomes apparent, for instance, in the desire for proximity with an attachment figure, the comfort (or discomfort) experienced in their presence , distress upon involuntary separation, or the grief that may be caused by losing them. Moreover, dispositions for attachment can be considered central to the idiosyncratic ways in which individuals tend to emotionally affect and respond to others, and also to the specific patterns of deploying and regulating emotions in relationships and beyond. Such patterns are most fundamentally established through early socialization, as caregivers and other social partners may play a powerful role in amplifying, curbing, or modulating a child's affective environment and their various affective responses to it.
This article examines the parent intervention program evaluated by Weber et al. (2017) and argues that there are scientific and ethical problems with such intervention efforts in applied developmental science. Scientifically , these... more
This article examines the parent intervention program evaluated by Weber et al. (2017) and argues that there are scientific and ethical problems with such intervention efforts in applied developmental science. Scientifically , these programs rely on data from a small and narrow sample of the world's population; assume the existence of fixed developmental pathways; and pit scientific knowledge against indigenous knowledge. The authors question the critical role of talk as solely providing the rich cognitive stimulation important to school success, and the critical role of primary caregivers as teachers of children's verbal competency. Ethically, these programs do not sufficiently explore how an intervention in one aspect of child care will affect the commu-nity's culturally organized patterns of child care.
The present study investigated the impact of young adolescents' learning environment on their culturally mediated view of themselves as expressed in their self-drawings. The sample consisted of 83 young adolescents, 35 male and 48 female,... more
The present study investigated the impact of young adolescents' learning environment on their culturally mediated view of themselves as expressed in their self-drawings. The sample consisted of 83 young adolescents, 35 male and 48 female, aged 10-14 years with an overall average of 12 years living in three diverse ecosocial contexts within the Southern region of Madagascar: 28 participants were recruited in rural villages, 14 adolescents lived in a small town, and 41 adolescents were raised in a large city. The participants did not differ in age or gender distribution. The analyses revealed significant differences in the adolescents' emotional expressions, drawing styles, visual appearances, and figure sizes in their self-depictions, which were in line with ecosocial variations in their learning environments. The findings are interpreted in light of the participants' varying socialization experiences, access to formal education, and exposure to modern media and a Western lifestyle.
This panel calls for papers on parenting interventions, i.e. institutionalized measures aiming at re-educating parents, specifically in transcultural, transnational or minoritarian settings. It focuses on the political and affective... more
This panel calls for papers on parenting interventions, i.e. institutionalized measures aiming at re-educating parents, specifically in transcultural, transnational or minoritarian settings. It focuses on the political and affective dynamics, patterns, and conflicts involved in these interventions.
This article considers claims of Mesman et al. (2017) that sensitive responsiveness as defined by Ainsworth, while not uniformly expressed across cultural contexts, is universal. Evidence presented demonstrates that none of the components... more
This article considers claims of Mesman et al. (2017) that sensitive responsiveness as defined by Ainsworth, while not uniformly expressed across cultural contexts, is universal. Evidence presented demonstrates that none of the components of sensitive responsiveness (i.e., which partner takes the lead, whose point of view is primary , and the turn-taking structure of interactions) or warmth are universal. Mesman and colleagues' proposal that sensitive responsiveness is " providing for infant needs " is critiqued. Constructs concerning caregiver quality must be embedded within a nexus of cultural logic, including caregiving practices, based on ecologically valid childrearing values and beliefs. Sensitive responsiveness, as defined by Mesman and attachment theorists, is not universal. Attachment theory and cultural or cross-cultural psychology are not built on common ground.
This article explores ethical issues raised by parenting interventions implemented in communities in low-to middle-income countries (LMICs) with rural, subsistence lifestyles. Many of these interventions foster " positive parenting... more
This article explores ethical issues raised by parenting interventions implemented in communities in low-to middle-income countries (LMICs) with rural, subsistence lifestyles. Many of these interventions foster " positive parenting practices " to improve children's chances of fulfilling their developmental potential. The practices are derived from attachment theory and presented as the universal standard of good care. But attachment-based parenting is typical primarily of people living Western lifestyles and runs counter to the different ways many people with other lifestyles care for their children given what they want for them. Thus, such parenting interventions involve encouraging caregivers to change their practices and views, usually with little understanding of how such changes affect child, family, and community. This undermines researchers' and practitioners' ability to honor promises to uphold ethic codes of respect and beneficence. Support for this claim is provided by comparing positive parenting practices advocated by the United Nations International Children's Emergency Fund (UNICEF; with the world health organization [WHO]) Care for Child Development (CCD) intervention with parenting practices typical of communities with rural, subsistence lifestyles—the most common of lifestyles worldwide and largely observed in LMICs. As UNICEF has a considerable presence in these countries, the CCD intervention was selected as a case study. In addition, parenting interventions typically target people who are poor, and the issues this raises regarding ethics of fairness and justice are considered. Recommendations are made for ways change agents can
Attachment Theory has its roots in an ethnocentric complex of ideas, longstanding in the United States, under the rubric of "intensive mothering." Among these various approaches and programs, attachment theory has had an inordinate... more
Attachment Theory has its roots in an ethnocentric complex of ideas, longstanding in the United States, under the rubric of "intensive mothering." Among these various approaches and programs, attachment theory has had an inordinate influence on a wide range of professions concerned with children (family therapy, education, the legal system, and public policy, the medical profession, etc.) inside and outside the United States. This chapter looks critically at how attachment theory has been applied in a variety of contexts and discusses its influence on parenting. It examines the distortion that often results when research findings are translated into actual applications or programs, ignoring any particularities of cultural context, It describes how attachment theory has been used as the basis for child-rearing manuals and has influenced programs and policies more directly, to form legal decisions that affect families, as well as to develop public policy and programs - all without requisite evidence to support such application and, more importantly, without regard to cultural context. Because child-rearing practices vary among cultures, the value systems that motivate these different practices must be recognized and accounted for when applications are developed and implemented. It concludes with a call for researchers to become proactive in rectifying misuses of attachment theory and holds that doing so is a matter of social responsibility.
ABSTRACT This chapter presents an alternative view to classic attachment theory and research, arguing for systematic, ethnographically informed, approaches to the study of child development. It begins with the observation that the... more
ABSTRACT This chapter presents an alternative view to classic attachment theory and research, arguing for systematic, ethnographically informed, approaches to the study of child development. It begins with the observation that the attachments children develop are locally determined, and insists that these features of attachment can only be captured through observing, talking with, and listening to local people as they go about living their lives, including caring for children. It reviews the profound ways in which childcare around the world differs from the Western model, upon which attachment theory was founded and myriad recommendations have been derived. This worldwide account of childcare is profusely illustrated with ethnographic examples. Network theory is then discussed: from the full range of social networks to relational ones (i.e., smaller sets of individuals to whom children may become attached). The chapter considers attachment theorists’ resistance to the idea of multiple attachments, historically and still today.  Discussion closes with a summary of the implications of our theoretical rethinking and the questions that remain.
This contribution investigates sentiments of justice in a rural community of Southern Madagascar in relation to the police (gendarmerie). Starting from the observation that gendarmes are commonly feared by the village population, it seeks... more
This contribution investigates sentiments of justice in a rural community of Southern Madagascar in relation to the police (gendarmerie). Starting from the observation that gendarmes are commonly feared by the village population, it seeks to explore how this widespread sentiment emerges, stabilizes, and transforms through interactions between gendarmes, villagers, urbanites, and multiple legal frames. On the micro level ‘police fright’ is closely interlinked with the image of gendarmes as powerful strangers, which can be traced back to colonial times and is constantly re-instantiated by villager-gendarme interactions. At a broader level it is argued that the villagers tend to position the gendarmes beyond their hierarchically structured moral community, within an egalitarian social sphere based on antagonistic reciprocity. Against this backdrop the question is raised, if and under which conditions these sentiments towards gendarmes may alter. To this end a second group of actors is taken into account: formally educated relatives of the villagers in a nearby town. Referring to the normative frame of the state, these actors display indignation upon the gendarme’s “power abuse” and sometime support the villagers by taking legal measures. Their efforts of encouraging the villagers to overcome their fear has had hardly any effect. It is argued that the sentiments and their normative reference are anchored in broader socio-emotional repertoires acquired in the process of socialization. While the villager’s sentiments towards gendarmes are based in the socialization within a particular peer culture, the urbanites draw in addition on their education in institutions of the state.
ABSTRACT Although much has been written about “socialization” and “emotion” in different cultures, these two topics have rarely been systematically combined and analyzed from the perspective of ontogenetic development. Drawing on... more
ABSTRACT Although much has been written about “socialization” and “emotion” in different cultures, these two topics have rarely been systematically combined and analyzed from the perspective of ontogenetic development. Drawing on approaches from developmental psychology and cultural anthropology, we examine how cultural models of emotion and their corresponding emotionally arousing child-rearing practices lead to culture-specific pathways of emotion development among the Bara (Madagascar) and the Tao (Taiwan). We focus especially on socializing emotions, which we define in terms of their function to orient individuals toward cultural norms and values and to inculcate social conformity. Based on empirical data gathered in long-term ethnographic field studies, we provide evidence that the Bara emphasize “fear” and the Tao “anxiety” as focal socializing emotions.
Research on childhood in anthropology and neighboring disciplines has continuously broadened the range of the social partners that are considered relevant for young children's development-from parents to other caregivers, siblings, and... more
Research on childhood in anthropology and neighboring disciplines has continuously broadened the range of the social partners that are considered relevant for young children's development-from parents to other caregivers, siblings, and peers. Yet most studies as well as interventions in early childhood still focus exclusively on parents, who are presumed to be the most significant socializing agents. Objecting to such a hierarchical understanding of the social world of children, I propose a complementarity view. Rather than being linearly ranked in a hierarchy of significance, children's social partners may complement each other by providing different but equally significant experiences. My suggestions are based on an ethnographic study in a rural community in Madagascar. Focusing on children in the first 3 years of life, I explore the full range of their social partners and the respective experiences they provide. Caregivers focus on children's physical needs and aim to keep them in a calm emotional state, while other young related children are the most crucial partners when it comes to play, face-to-face interaction, and the exchange of intense emotions. These complementary roles, I argue, lead to the parallel formation of two distinct socioemotional modes: a hierarchical one and an egalitarian one.